دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 31196
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

آلکسیتیمیا و ارتباط آن با چک کردن بدن و تصویر بدن در نمونه ها زن غیربالینی

عنوان انگلیسی
Alexithymia and its relationships with body checking and body image in a non-clinical female sample
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
31196 2007 9 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Eating Behaviors, Volume 8, Issue 3, August 2007, Pages 296–304

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
آلکسی تایمیا - چک کردن بدن - تصویر بدن - اختلالات اشتها - احساسات -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Alexithymia, Body checking, Body image, Eating disorders, Emotions,
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  آلکسیتیمیا و ارتباط آن با چک کردن بدن و تصویر بدن در نمونه ها زن غیربالینی

چکیده انگلیسی

The aim of the present study was to evaluate in a non-clinical sample of undergraduate women, the relationships between alexithymia, body checking and body image, identifying predictive factors associated with the possible risk of developing an Eating Disorder (ED). The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), Body Checking Questionnaire (BCQ), Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26), Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ), Interaction Anxiousness Scale (IAS), Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) were completed by 254 undergraduate females. We found that alexithymics had more consistent body checking behaviors and higher body dissatisfaction than nonalexithymics. In addition, alexithymics also reported a higher potential risk for ED (higher scores on EAT-26) when compared to nonalexithymics. Difficulty in identifying and describing feelings subscales of TAS-20, Overall appearance and Specific Body Parts subscales of BCQ as well as lower self-esteem was associated with higher ED risk in a linear regression analysis. Thus, a combination of alexithymia, low self-esteem, body checking behaviors and body dissatisfaction may be a risk factor for symptoms of ED at least in a non-clinical sample of university women.

مقدمه انگلیسی

It is widely recognized that the body dissatisfaction and an excessive concern about body weight and shape are core characteristic of Eating Disorders (ED) and are used to determine self-worth (Fairburn et al., 2003 and Garner et al., 1984). Recently, there was an increased interest about the body image as a multidimensional issue that involves perceptual, attitudinal and behavioral characteristics (Cash and Henry, 1995, Ehrsson et al., 2005, Williamson, 1990 and Williamson, 1996). Many researchers have focused their attention mainly to the perceptual and attitudinal aspects of body image whereas only few studies have investigated the behavioral consequences related to a negative body image (Reas, Whisenhunt, Netemeyer, & Williamson, 2002). Patients with or at risk of ED often have a negative perception of several body parts; in some cases, they avoid social situations that may point out their physical appearance and exert a ritualistic checking on their body weight and shape (Reas et al., 2002 and Toro et al., 2005). The body checking could be considered somewhat similar to compulsive behaviors; through this checking patients are often able to avoid the anxiety that derives from negative concerns about their body weight and shape (Rosen, 1997 and Williamson, 1990). Examples of body checking are the repetitive measure of body weight, the frequent exposures at mirror in order to verify possible body shape changes, the use of particular clothes that can “measure” the fatness or the thinness, the pinching of several body parts to verify their consistence, the comparison with other people about the own body weight and shape, the checking to see if thighs rub together and many more (Fairburn et al., 2003 and Reas et al., 2002). On the other hand, in ED patients, paradoxically, the body checking may reinforce the body dissatisfaction focusing further attention on concerns related to a negative body image (Fairburn, 1997). Fairburn, Shafran, and Cooper (1999) have pointed out the rule of the “body control” in the clinical evolution of anorexia. The body control and checking are used to monitor the body weight and shape changes, but increase the perceived imperfections and may lead to a higher body weight control. As consequence, a hypervigilant body control preserves the negative beliefs about presumed abnormal body shape. Moreover, they have hypothesized that the normal variations of body weight may be directly related to mood swings in ED patients. Since the body control and checking may play a role in the development and maintenance of an ED, the evaluation of the body checking behaviors may be useful in therapeutic programs aimed to help patients with ED (Bowers, 2000 and Garner and Garfinkel, 1997). In fact, as clinical observations indicate that body checking increase both the patient's preoccupation with body shape and weight and the motivation to maintain dietary restraint, a therapeutic program aimed to reducing body checking may contribute to reduce body dissatisfaction and, consequently, ED symptoms (Shafran, Fairburn, Robinson, & Lask, 2004). Furthermore, it is reasonable to think that individuals with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa develop a highly organized cognitive schema concerning body- and weight-related information (Williamson, Muller, Reas, & Thaw, 1999). Body checking behaviors, like social scanning, may be both positively and negatively reinforced and strengthen the concerns about body size and shape as well as food and eating. Body checking rituals are used to regulate emotions through confirmation or attenuation of fears (and are therefore negatively reinforced), whereas in many circumstances the checking behaviors reinforce disordered patterns of behavior when the ritual results in an unfavourable or negative perception (Williamson, White, York-Crowe, & Stewart, 2004). Coined by Sifneos (1973), the term “alexithymia” was introduced to designate a cluster of cognitive and affective characteristics that were observed among patients with psychosomatic diseases. The alexithymia construct, formulated from clinical investigations, is multifaceted and includes four distinct characteristics: (a) difficulty in identifying and describing feelings, (b) difficulty in distinguishing feelings from the bodily sensations, (c) diminution of fantasy, and (d) concrete and poorly introspective thinking (Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1991). Alexithymic individuals have affective dysregulation, the inability to self soothe and manage emotions because of a lack of awareness of emotions (Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997). Thus, the adaptive informational value of emotions that is important for emotion regulation often eludes these individuals. In non-clinical samples, the prevalence of alexithymia ranges from 0% to 28% (Guilbaud et al., 2002 and Jimerson et al., 1994). An increasing body of research indicates that alexithymia features exist not only in classic psychosomatic disorders but also in other severe and chronic somatic diseases and psychiatric disorders such as Somatoform Disorders, Major Depression and other Axis I disorders (Bankier et al., 2001 and De Berardis et al., 2005). Concerning EDs, Bruch (1973) suggested that the difficulty to distinguish and describe feelings is one of the main problems in ED patients, related to a sense of general inadequacy and a lack of control over one's life. Moreover, Bruch (1962) proposed that in anorexia nervosa an approach that helps patients to become aware of and identify inner states, including emotions, would be useful. It is known that alexithymia may play an important role in EDs: specifically alexithymics patients may show a higher psychological distress than nonalexithymics (Taylor, Parker, Bagby, & Bourke, 1996) and the presence of an alexithymic trait may be related to a higher severity of EDs themselves (Bydlowski et al., 2005 and Carano et al., 2006). Furthermore, there are evidences that ED patients are considerably more alexithymic than apparently healthy controls (Cochrane et al., 1993 and Schmidt et al., 1993) and some studies have specified that alexithymia is more related to the psychological characteristics of patients with EDs than to the eating behavior itself (Bydlowski et al., 2005, de Groot et al., 1995 and Taylor et al., 1996). It is reported that emotional expression may be inversely related to body dissatisfaction (Hayaki, Friedman, & Brownell, 2002). However, concerning non-clinical samples, in an interesting and well conducted study, Quinton and Wagner (2005) found that alexithymia did not predict neither total EAT-26 score, nor two EDI-2 subscales measuring aspects of eating psychopathology. They concluded that, although disturbed emotional functioning is a feature of eating disorders, it did not relate directly to the core psychopathology. Some core aspects of alexithymic construct, as a difficulty in distinguishing emotional states from bodily sensations, may be more characterized in patients with EDs and a possible explanation might be that ED patients may appear dramatically and deeply incapable of being in touch with their inner emotive world (Andersen, 1988). As consequence, these subjects may focus their attention on negative perceptual aspects of body bypassing emotional experiences (Montebarocci et al., 2006). Taken together, these findings may suggest that alexithymia, body checking and body-image disturbances may be strongly correlated in EDs. In our opinion, the body checking on an alexithymia background can create or exacerbate body-image problems. So, the aim of the present study was to evaluate in a non-clinical sample of female university students the relationships between alexithymia, body checking and body image, testing whether alexithymics present higher psychological distress and identifying possible predictive factors associated with the possible risk of developing an ED. This may have important clinical implications especially concerning cognitive aspects related to EDs.