دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 33691
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

MRI ساختاری زمینه بالا نشان می دهد حافظه اپیزودیک مخصوص در زیر شاخه هیپوکامپ در ارتباط است

عنوان انگلیسی
High field structural MRI reveals specific episodic memory correlates in the subfields of the hippocampus
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
33691 2014 13 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Neuropsychologia, Volume 53, January 2014, Pages 233–245

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
هیپوکامپ - حافظه اپیزودیک - ساختاری آزمون حافظه وکسلر - لوب تمپورال داخلی - رشته های فرعی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Hippocampus; Episodic memory; Structural MRI; Wechsler Memory Scale; Medial temporal lobe; Subfields
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  MRI ساختاری زمینه بالا نشان می دهد حافظه اپیزودیک مخصوص در زیر شاخه هیپوکامپ در ارتباط است

چکیده انگلیسی

The involvement of the hippocampus (HC) in episodic memory is well accepted; however it is unclear how each subfield within the HC contributes to memory function. Recent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies suggest differential involvement of hippocampal subfields and subregions in episodic memory. However, most structural MRI studies have examined the HC subfields within a single subregion of the HC and used specialised experimental memory paradigms. The purpose of the present study was to determine the association between volumes of HC subfields throughout the entire HC structure and performance on standard neuropsychological memory tests in a young, healthy population. We recruited 34 healthy participants under the age of 50. MRI data was acquired with a fast spin echo (FSE) sequence yielding a 0.52×0.68×1.0 mm3 native resolution. The HC subfields – the cornu ammonis 1–3 (CA), dentate gyrus (DG), and subiculum (SUB) – were segmented manually within three hippocampal subregions using a previously defined protocol. Participants were administered the Wechsler Memory Scale, 4th edition (WMS-IV) to assess performance in episodic memory using verbal (Logical Memory, LM) and visual (Designs, DE; visual-spatial memory, DE-Spatial; visual-content memory, DE-Content) memory subtests. Working memory subtests (Spatial Addition, SA; and Symbol Span, SSP) were included as well. Working memory was not associated with any HC volumes. Volumes of the DG were correlated with verbal memory (LM) and visual-spatial memory (DE-Spatial). Posterior CA volumes correlated with both visual-spatial and visual-object memory (DE-Spatial, DE-Content). In general, anterior subregion volumes (HC head) correlated with verbal memory, while some anterior and many posterior HC subregion volumes (body and tail) correlated with visual memory scores (DE-Spatial, DE-Content). In addition, while verbal memory showed left-lateralized associations with HC volumes, visual memory was associated with HC volumes bilaterally. This the first study to examine the associations between hippocampal subfield volumes across the entire hippocampal formation with performance in a set of standard memory tasks.

مقدمه انگلیسی

It has been over half a century since the famous case of patient H.M. and his bilateral medial temporal lobe (MTL) resections gave psychologists strong evidence that the MTL is required for learning and memory (Scoville & Milner, 1957). Subsequent studies of the hippocampus (HC) – a bilateral structure within the MTL – have found that volumetric measurements taken via Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) can predict performance on a number of common neuropsychological tests and episodic memory paradigms. This is true for healthy controls with intact HC tissue (Foster et al., 1999, Hackert et al., 2002, Convit et al., 2003 and Rosen et al., 2003) and in patients with temporal lobectomies (Scoville and Milner, 1957 and Loring et al., 1993). This relationship can also be seen in pathological instances where reduced HC volume is highly correlated with poorer memory performance, such as in Alzheimer's disease; (Köhler et al., 1998), schizophrenia (Seidman et al., 2002 and O’Driscoll et al., 2001), and hippocampal sclerosis observed in temporal lobe epilepsy (Lencz et al., 1992, Baxendale et al., 1998, Pegna et al., 2002, Griffith et al., 2003 and Griffith et al., 2004). In healthy populations with intact HC tissue, however, the correlation between the volume of the hippocampus and its corresponding function are usually less consistent (Rodrigue and Raz, 2004 and Van Petten, 2004). It seems that these relationships are most obvious in individuals with significant cognitive impairments from neurological disorders or age-related neurodegeneration which result in a reduction of HC volume. It is less clear whether HC volume–memory associations are detectable in a young, healthy population. We know from selective lesion studies in rats that the HC is functionally subdivided along the septotemporal axis into dorsal and ventral regions, with each region being associated with a distinct set of behaviors (Bannerman et al., 2004). It has been suggested that the dorsal HC – referred to as the posterior HC in primates – has a preferential role in spatial learning. The ventral HC – equivalent to the primate anterior HC – may have a preferential role in anxiety-related behaviors (Bannerman et al., 2004). In humans, the HC is subdivided along the anteroposterior axis into subregions. These subregions as we will refer to them are the HC head, body, and tail (Duvernoy, 2005), listed from most anterior to most posterior. In studies which examine the HC in just two anteroposterior subdivisions, it has been suggested that the anterior HC may have a preferential role in anxiety-related behaviors and verbal memory, while the posterior HC has a preferential role in spatial learning. Specifically, in a structural MRI study, Szeszko et al. (2006) found that in healthy individuals stress correlated most strongly with anterior HC volume. HC head size is positively correlated with word learning (Hackert et al., 2002), and similarly Chen, Chuah, Sim, & Chee (2010) found that greater anterior HC volumes were associated with delayed verbal memory. Posterior HC volumes correlate with spatial memory, both in Chen et al. (2010), and Woollett and Maguire (2011), who demonstrated in a longitudinal study that successfully certified taxi drivers had an increase in posterior HC grey matter volumes, compared to controls and apprentices who did not pass their qualifying exam. The functional specificity of these studies conceivably reflects an underlying structural differentiation along the anteroposterior axis. Specifically, the afferent and efferent external connectivity changes along this axis (Andersen, Bliss, & Skrede, 1971). The entorhinal cortex projects well-defined regions along the anteroposterior axis of the dentate gyrus (DG) (Dolorfo & Amaral, 1998), with input from the sensory cortex via entorhinal cortex entering mainly in the dorsal (posterior) two-thirds of the DG. This organization is similar for efferent connections back to cortical areas. Axons from cornu ammonis (CA) and subiculum (SUB) synapse onto the entorhinal cortex in a lateral-to-medial direction (Köhler, 1985, van Groen et al., 1986 and van Groen and Wyss, 1990). For a more detailed overview, see Moser and Moser (1998). If we consider this anteroposterior gradient of connective distinctiveness, we can infer that the HC head, body, and tail will have distinct roles in cognition. The HC head shows connections to amygdala, and uncinate fasciculus white matter which reciprocally projects to temporal pole, insula, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Kier, Staib, Davis, & Bronen, 2004). In contrast, resting-state fMRI has correlated posterior HC activity with both anterior and posterior cingulate, precuneus, and inferior parietal cortex (Poppenk & Moscovitch, 2011). Overall, the extant literature illustrates a specialization of hippocampal subregions, a finding which could be overlooked when examining the HC as a whole. For example Poppenk and Moscovitch (2011) reported that better performance on tasks of word pair, word-picture pairings and scene recognition was associated with positively with posterior and negatively with anterior HC volume, while overall HC volume was not sensitive to these measures. Therefore, separating a global volume of the HC into connectively-distinct subregions in studies of relationships between hippocampal volume and memory is vital. Apart from anteroposterior subregion divisions, the HC has a heterogeneous inner structure that can be further delineated in MR images into several histologically-defined subfields, including the cornu ammonis (CA1-3), dentate gyrus (DG), and subiculum (SUB) (for a full review, see Duvernoy, 2005). Each subfield houses a distinct population of neurons with distinct connectivity, suggesting a cytological basis for differential vulnerability of the subfields to different pathological processes. For example, severe stress can lead to a massive loss of CA3 pyramidal cells in the HC of rats and monkeys (Sapolsky, 2000), neuronal loss in CA1 is typical for Alzheimer's disease (West, Coleman, Flood, & Troncoso, 1994), and DG and CA subfields are affected in major depression (Stockmeier et al., 2004). In the trisynaptic circuit, afferent excitatory cortical inputs via entorhinal cortex pass through the perforant path to the granule cells of the DG. Mossy fibers arising from DG travel to CA3/4 before passing through remaining CA subfields via Shaffer collaterals then SUB, or passing directly from CA3/4 to the fimbria and on to the fornix. Alternatively, the direct intrahippocampal pathway receives input “directly” from entorhinal cortex to CA then SUB (Duvernoy, 2005). These pathways are largely conserved along the anteroposterior axis, in contrast to the afferent and efferent connections mentioned above (Andersen et al., 1971). This cytological distinctiveness between subfields could also be a basis for differential roles in memory. In rodents, studies examining immediate early gene activation in hippocampal subfields can reveal this functional heterogeneity. Increased c-fos activation in CA, but not DG or SUB corresponds to novel arrangements of familiar items (Wan, Aggleton, & Brown, 1999), and in dorsal hippocampus this corresponds with novel landmarks in a radial maze task (Vann, Brown, Erichsen, & Aggleton, 2000). In the DG, levels of another immediate early gene, Zif268, are correlated with spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze after only two training sessions, whereas CA expression of Zif268 is correlated with performance after five training sessions (Poirier, Amin, & Aggleton, 2008). Together this suggests a model of functional heterogeneity of subfields that is not only task-specific, but is also temporally-dependent. Human studies of specialized roles of hippocampal subfields in memory are limited to a few recent high resolution (<2 mm in-plane) fMRI studies (Bakker et al., 2008, Das et al., 2011, Eldridge et al., 2005, Preston et al., 2010, Suthana et al., 2011, Yassa et al., 2011 and Zeineh et al., 2003) and ultra-high resolution (<1 mm in-plane) high field structural MRI studies (Kerchner et al., 2012, Mueller et al., 2011 and Mueller et al., 2012). In terms of encoding, input structures such as the DG, CA2, and CA3 are most active during this phase, while CA1, an output structure, is most active during successful retrieval (Eldridge et al., 2005 and Zeineh et al., 2003). Bakker et al. (2008) found that a pattern completion task (the ability to recall a stored memory pattern in response to a degraded observation of elements of the stored pattern) elicited a blood–oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response in the CA1 and SUB, while pattern separation (the ability to keep distinct memory patterns separate) was associated with CA3/DG activity. More recently, this group reported an age-related reduction in pattern separation capacity in humans associated with both functional changes in CA3/DG activity and structural deterioration in the perforant path and CA3/DG area (Yassa et al., 2011). Preston et al. (2010) observed functional activation to both novel faces and scenes in all MTL regions. However, similar percentages of voxels were sensitive to novel faces and scenes in perirhinal cortex, entorhinal cortex and a combined region comprising the DG and CA2-3 subfields, whereas parahippocampal cortex, CA1 and SUB demonstrated greater sensitivity to novel scene than face stimuli. Suthana et al. (2011) reported increased activity within right CA2-3/DG sub-region during encoding compared to retrieval of spatial associations. In contrast, right subicular activation increased during retrieval compared to encoding. Group differences in activation of hippocampal subfields were also detected in TLE patients, with the DG and the anterior HC region showing the greatest activation differences between groups (Das et al., 2011). Despite the fact that recent fMRI studies have found functional specialization of hippocampal subfields in different aspects of memory such as pattern separation, face/scene encoding, encoding and retrieval of spatial associations, and verbal memory, an integration of these findings remains difficult due to the variety of techniques used to delineate the HC subfields and the specific memory paradigms used. Thus, the current applicability of fMRI evidence of HC subfield involvement in memory to clinical populations, usually assessed with neuropsychological memory test batteries, remains limited. Inferences from structural MRI studies regarding potential subregion-specific structural involvement of the HC in memory cannot easily be drawn either, especially considering that the studies on the level of HC subfield resolution were mainly acquired in elderly individuals (Mueller et al., 2011) or those with temporal lobe epilepsy (Mueller et al., 2012). In high field structural MRI studies, insights confirming the subfield contributions to memory have also been found. Mueller et al. (2011) performed a comparison of subfield volumes and performance on the California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II) in a combined group of healthy controls and cognitively impaired elderly individuals. It was found that CA3 and DG volume positively correlated with both immediate and short free recall discriminability, and that CA1 positively correlated with delayed recall discriminability. In a second related study with similar results Mueller et al. (2012) found that in a combined group of healthy controls and patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, immediate auditory recall performance was associated with larger CA3/DG volumes, and that delayed auditory recall was associated with larger CA1 volumes. Measurements in these studies were taken from within the hippocampal body only, and so the relationship of subfields in the HC head or tail to episodic memory remains unclear. However in a study which summed subfield volumes across the head, body, and parts of the tail, CA1 was also associated with delayed verbal learning in Alzheimer's patients (Kerchner et al., 2012), suggesting this association can be found in both healthy and clinical populations. The combination of an in vivo volumetric analysis of subfields across the entire HC with standard neuropsychological memory tests could provide important insights into whether memory processes are associated with the volume of particular HC subfields. In addition, it is not clear whether such volume–function correlations are detectable in a younger, healthy population. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to determine associations between volumes of hippocampal subfields and performance on standard memory tests in a population of young, healthy individuals. The Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-IV; Pearson Education I., 2009) was chosen for this study as it includes tasks designed to assess immediate and delayed memory in visuospatial and verbal tasks. Based on the aforementioned high-field fMRI studies and structural MRI studies, we hypothesized that performance on these visuospatial tasks will be associated with the volumes of the CA and DG subfields in the posterior regions of the HC (i.e., body and tail), and performance on verbal tasks will be associated with CA and DG in the anterior HC (HC head).