دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 5075
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

عجله به عنوان انگیزش کلیدی در گردشگری ماجراجویانه ماهر : حل خطر ابتلا به تناقض تفریح ​​و سرگرمی

عنوان انگلیسی
Rush as a key motivation in skilled adventure tourism: Resolving the risk recreation paradox
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
5075 2012 10 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Tourism Management, Volume 33, Issue 4, August 2012, Pages 961–970

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
هیجان - جریان - به اوج خود رسیدن - لبه کار - احساس - بازار یابی
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  عجله به عنوان انگیزش کلیدی در گردشگری ماجراجویانه ماهر : حل خطر ابتلا به تناقض تفریح ​​و سرگرمی

چکیده انگلیسی

At least 14 different motivations for adventure tourism and recreation, some internal and some external, have been identified in ∼50 previous studies. Skilled adventure practitioners refer to ineffable experiences, comprehensible only to other participants and containing a strong emotional component. These are also reflected in the popular literature of adventure tourism. This contribution draws on >2000 person-days of ethnographic and autoethnographic experience to formalise this particular category of experience as rush. To the practitioner, rush is a single tangible experience. To the analyst, it may be seen as the simultaneous experience of flow and thrill. Experiences which provide rush are often risky, but it is rush rather than risk which provides the attraction. Rush is addictive and never guaranteed, but the chance of rush is sufficient motivation to buy adventure tours.

مقدمه انگلیسی

1.1. Significance The motivations of adventure tourists are significant for tourism both as a category of human behaviour and as a trillion-dollar global industry. From a social science perspective, adventure tour clients make conscious choices to allocate discretionary time and funds to adventure activities, with no material gain. Their reasons and rewards for doing so thus provide insights into human psychology (Arnould and Price, 1993, Arnould et al., 1999, Crompton, 1979, Holyfield, 1999, Holyfield and Fine, 1997 and Jonas, 1999). From the commercial perspective, knowledge of clients’ motivations helps tour operators construct products (Buckley, 2007), design marketing strategies (Buckley, 2003, Gilbert and Hudson, 2000 and Williams and Soutar, 2009), choreograph client experiences (Arnould et al., 1999, Holyfield, 1999, Holyfield and Fine, 1997, Jonas, 1999, Pomfret, 2006 and Sharpe, 2005), and defend accident lawsuits (Yerger, 2004–2005). 1.2. Risk recreation paradox Adventure tourists pay for risk recreation activities (Breivik, 1996, Lipscombe, 2007 and Page et al., 2005), but adventure tour operators aim to minimise risks (Buckley, 2006, Cater, 2006 and Morgan, 2010). The orthodox response to this paradox (Buckley, 2010a and Cater, 2006) is that adventure tour operators sell their clients the semblance of risk so as to confer social capital (Bartkus and Davis, 2009 and McGillivray and Frew, 2007), whilst protecting them from real risk so as to avoid illness and injury, medical and legal costs, and poor publicity. This contribution proposes that the orthodox resolution applies only to adventure tour products designed for unskilled clients. At the skilled end of the adventure tourism spectrum, I argue that clients are in fact motivated not by risk but by a particular type of experience referred to here as rush. This appears to be the first formal recognition, description and analysis of rush. Sensations which seem to correspond to rush, as defined here, have been mentioned by participants in some previous studies, but as “ineffable” or “indescribable” (Allman et al., 2009 and Bratton et al., 1979). Thus Lyng and Snow (1986) refer to the “admonition that ‘if you want to know what it’s like, then do it’”. Brymer and Oades (2009) quote a BASE jumper: “you can’t even begin to try to make someone who hasn’t done it understand”. And Bratton et al. (1979, p. 24) use a classic quotation from “the immortal Mallory”: “if you have to ask why men (sic) climb, you wouldn’t understand the answer.” For active participants in adventure tourism, rush is a clear, distinct and self-contained concept. It is widespread in the marketing materials of adventure tourism and the popular literature of adventure recreation. Examples include: a wingsuit skydiver quoted by Midol and Broyer (1995); snowboard racer Jeremy Jones, quoted by Heino (2000); or skateboard freestylist Mat Hoffman, quoted by Higgins (2010). It is a common component in conversation between clients on commercial adventure tours. In this contribution I argue that rush can be understood as a formal academic concept, despite claims by many adventure exponents that it can only be appreciated if experienced in person. 1.3. Approach This contribution aims to communicate and formalise a concept which previous authors, both popular and academic, have referred to as comprehensible only to those who have experienced it, and indescribable to those who have not. The approach taken here is thus principally autoethnographic, i.e. drawn from the author’s own experiences. If, in the views of previous authors, personal experience is critical to comprehension, then an autoethnographic basis is the only approach available. It is also ethnographic, drawn from extended lived experience as part of a set of subcultural groups, the exponents of various forms of skilled outdoor recreation. There are many types of ethnography and autoethnography, and all of them balance the details of record against the depth of involvement. Approaches which involve interviews, recording devices and note-taking visible to participants may bias their behaviour and restrict their conversation, and interfere with the researcher’s ability to identify with their experiences. The most fundamental autoethnographic approach involves living as one of those under study, and reflecting and recording only when such interaction and immersion is not under way. This is the method adopted here. The approach is also analytic, in the sense that it attempts to identify key aspects of those experiences, distil them to irreducible components, and show their relationships to pre-existing concepts. Finally, it contains an unusual element, an attempt to communicate this supposedly indescribable experience to readers who may not themselves have lived through it. The communication mechanism is a set of short descriptive vignettes. This is more closely analogous to creative writing or drama than to academic analysis. It is a routine component of novels or movies, for example, which aim to generate emotional empathy in their audience, for circumstances which the audience have not themselves experienced in person. In the technical academic literature of tourism research, however, materials are commonly used only as a basis for content or discourse analysis. Autoethnographic approaches have been rather little used in tourism research. Ryan, 2005 and Ryan and Stewart, 2009 and Buckley, 2006, Buckley, 2010a and Buckley, 2010b) do effectively use such approaches, but not explicitly. These approaches have a longer history in sport and leisure research (Allen-Collinson and Hockey, 2011, Ewert, 1985, Holyfield, 1999 and Irwin, 1973). Standard reference books on qualitative research methods, such as that by Silverman (2011) include ethnography but not autoethnography. In the literature of ethnography (Chang, 2008), there is a division between analytic autoethnography (Anderson, 2006), which uses the researcher’s experience as a source of data; and evocative autoethnography (Ellis, 2004), which uses creative writing to convey the emotional components of experience. This contribution uses both analytic and evocative approaches. As in all qualitative social science research, the author owes the reader a responsibility to reveal his or her own role in the group under study. Details are described in Section 4, but in brief, this article arose from the author’s experience as a participant in commercial adventure tours over the past 15 years. This included roles as client, guide, and ancillary staff. These roles were made possible through skills gained previously in individual outdoor recreation. They included extensive conversation with other participants, and familiarity with popular literature in the form of specialist outdoor recreation magazines, DVDs and marketing materials. This lived experience was not undertaken solely or specifically to study motivations, but it provides the raw material drawn upon here: the analytic autoethnographic approach advocated by Anderson (2006). The ethical concerns associated with autoethnographic approaches (Tolich, 2010) are avoided, as no person other than the author is quoted, described or identifiable in this text. As in many such studies, the components could be presented in a variety of different ways. The structure used here is as follows. First, previous literature on adventure motivations is reviewed, including a summary table identifying 14 distinct categories. Second, rush is defined as a formal concept, and distinguished from prior related concepts. Third, the circumstances providing autoethnographic experiences and ethnographic information on rush are outlined. Fourth, this information is summarised for five outdoor adventure activities, each including a vignette to communicate how rush is experienced by the participant. Fifth, findings are synthesised to present rush as an individual psychological experience, as a motivation in adventure tourism, and as a counterpart to risk. Under this structure, the analytical formalisation of rush is introduced before the autoethnographic components are presented. The reason for this order, rather than the reverse, is to situate rush relative to other relevant and related concepts such as flow and peak experience, and to provide context for the vignettes which illustrate rush from the participant perspective.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

1.1. Significance The motivations of adventure tourists are significant for tourism both as a category of human behaviour and as a trillion-dollar global industry. From a social science perspective, adventure tour clients make conscious choices to allocate discretionary time and funds to adventure activities, with no material gain. Their reasons and rewards for doing so thus provide insights into human psychology (Arnould and Price, 1993, Arnould et al., 1999, Crompton, 1979, Holyfield, 1999, Holyfield and Fine, 1997 and Jonas, 1999). From the commercial perspective, knowledge of clients’ motivations helps tour operators construct products (Buckley, 2007), design marketing strategies (Buckley, 2003, Gilbert and Hudson, 2000 and Williams and Soutar, 2009), choreograph client experiences (Arnould et al., 1999, Holyfield, 1999, Holyfield and Fine, 1997, Jonas, 1999, Pomfret, 2006 and Sharpe, 2005), and defend accident lawsuits (Yerger, 2004–2005). 1.2. Risk recreation paradox Adventure tourists pay for risk recreation activities (Breivik, 1996, Lipscombe, 2007 and Page et al., 2005), but adventure tour operators aim to minimise risks (Buckley, 2006, Cater, 2006 and Morgan, 2010). The orthodox response to this paradox (Buckley, 2010a and Cater, 2006) is that adventure tour operators sell their clients the semblance of risk so as to confer social capital (Bartkus and Davis, 2009 and McGillivray and Frew, 2007), whilst protecting them from real risk so as to avoid illness and injury, medical and legal costs, and poor publicity. This contribution proposes that the orthodox resolution applies only to adventure tour products designed for unskilled clients. At the skilled end of the adventure tourism spectrum, I argue that clients are in fact motivated not by risk but by a particular type of experience referred to here as rush. This appears to be the first formal recognition, description and analysis of rush. Sensations which seem to correspond to rush, as defined here, have been mentioned by participants in some previous studies, but as “ineffable” or “indescribable” (Allman et al., 2009 and Bratton et al., 1979). Thus Lyng and Snow (1986) refer to the “admonition that ‘if you want to know what it’s like, then do it’”. Brymer and Oades (2009) quote a BASE jumper: “you can’t even begin to try to make someone who hasn’t done it understand”. And Bratton et al. (1979, p. 24) use a classic quotation from “the immortal Mallory”: “if you have to ask why men (sic) climb, you wouldn’t understand the answer.” For active participants in adventure tourism, rush is a clear, distinct and self-contained concept. It is widespread in the marketing materials of adventure tourism and the popular literature of adventure recreation. Examples include: a wingsuit skydiver quoted by Midol and Broyer (1995); snowboard racer Jeremy Jones, quoted by Heino (2000); or skateboard freestylist Mat Hoffman, quoted by Higgins (2010). It is a common component in conversation between clients on commercial adventure tours. In this contribution I argue that rush can be understood as a formal academic concept, despite claims by many adventure exponents that it can only be appreciated if experienced in person. 1.3. Approach This contribution aims to communicate and formalise a concept which previous authors, both popular and academic, have referred to as comprehensible only to those who have experienced it, and indescribable to those who have not. The approach taken here is thus principally autoethnographic, i.e. drawn from the author’s own experiences. If, in the views of previous authors, personal experience is critical to comprehension, then an autoethnographic basis is the only approach available. It is also ethnographic, drawn from extended lived experience as part of a set of subcultural groups, the exponents of various forms of skilled outdoor recreation. There are many types of ethnography and autoethnography, and all of them balance the details of record against the depth of involvement. Approaches which involve interviews, recording devices and note-taking visible to participants may bias their behaviour and restrict their conversation, and interfere with the researcher’s ability to identify with their experiences. The most fundamental autoethnographic approach involves living as one of those under study, and reflecting and recording only when such interaction and immersion is not under way. This is the method adopted here. The approach is also analytic, in the sense that it attempts to identify key aspects of those experiences, distil them to irreducible components, and show their relationships to pre-existing concepts. Finally, it contains an unusual element, an attempt to communicate this supposedly indescribable experience to readers who may not themselves have lived through it. The communication mechanism is a set of short descriptive vignettes. This is more closely analogous to creative writing or drama than to academic analysis. It is a routine component of novels or movies, for example, which aim to generate emotional empathy in their audience, for circumstances which the audience have not themselves experienced in person. In the technical academic literature of tourism research, however, materials are commonly used only as a basis for content or discourse analysis. Autoethnographic approaches have been rather little used in tourism research. Ryan, 2005 and Ryan and Stewart, 2009 and Buckley, 2006, Buckley, 2010a and Buckley, 2010b) do effectively use such approaches, but not explicitly. These approaches have a longer history in sport and leisure research (Allen-Collinson and Hockey, 2011, Ewert, 1985, Holyfield, 1999 and Irwin, 1973). Standard reference books on qualitative research methods, such as that by Silverman (2011) include ethnography but not autoethnography. In the literature of ethnography (Chang, 2008), there is a division between analytic autoethnography (Anderson, 2006), which uses the researcher’s experience as a source of data; and evocative autoethnography (Ellis, 2004), which uses creative writing to convey the emotional components of experience. This contribution uses both analytic and evocative approaches. As in all qualitative social science research, the author owes the reader a responsibility to reveal his or her own role in the group under study. Details are described in Section 4, but in brief, this article arose from the author’s experience as a participant in commercial adventure tours over the past 15 years. This included roles as client, guide, and ancillary staff. These roles were made possible through skills gained previously in individual outdoor recreation. They included extensive conversation with other participants, and familiarity with popular literature in the form of specialist outdoor recreation magazines, DVDs and marketing materials. This lived experience was not undertaken solely or specifically to study motivations, but it provides the raw material drawn upon here: the analytic autoethnographic approach advocated by Anderson (2006). The ethical concerns associated with autoethnographic approaches (Tolich, 2010) are avoided, as no person other than the author is quoted, described or identifiable in this text. As in many such studies, the components could be presented in a variety of different ways. The structure used here is as follows. First, previous literature on adventure motivations is reviewed, including a summary table identifying 14 distinct categories. Second, rush is defined as a formal concept, and distinguished from prior related concepts. Third, the circumstances providing autoethnographic experiences and ethnographic information on rush are outlined. Fourth, this information is summarised for five outdoor adventure activities, each including a vignette to communicate how rush is experienced by the participant. Fifth, findings are synthesised to present rush as an individual psychological experience, as a motivation in adventure tourism, and as a counterpart to risk. Under this structure, the analytical formalisation of rush is introduced before the autoethnographic components are presented. The reason for this order, rather than the reverse, is to situate rush relative to other relevant and related concepts such as flow and peak experience, and to provide context for the vignettes which illustrate rush from the participant perspective.