دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 33593
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

اثر توجه تقسیم شده بر روی حافظه اپیزودیک در آسیب های مغزی مزمن: یک تابع شدت و استراتژی

عنوان انگلیسی
Effects of divided attention on episodic memory in chronic traumatic brain injury: a function of severity and strategy
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
33593 2002 17 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Neuropsychologia, Volume 40, Issue 13, 2002, Pages 2369–2385

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
- کنترل اجرایی - بارکننده لوب - نتیجه - نوروسایکولوژی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Executive control; Frontal-lobe; Outcome; Neuropsychology
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  اثر توجه تقسیم شده بر روی حافظه اپیزودیک در آسیب های مغزی مزمن: یک تابع شدت و استراتژی

چکیده انگلیسی

Eleven patients with mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI) and 13 patients with moderate-to-severe TBI (STBI) were compared to 10 matched controls on episodic memory for pictorial scene–object associations (e.g. kitchen–bread) and a range of standardized neuropsychological tests of memory and frontal-lobe functions. We tested the hypothesis that deficits in episodic memory result from impaired attentional resources and/or strategic control by manipulating attentional load at encoding (focused versus divided attention) and environmental support at retrieval (free recall and recalled cued by scene versus recognition of object and scene). Patients with TBI were disproportionately affected by the divided attention manipulation, but this effect was modulated by injury severity and encoding strategy. Overall, MTBI patients were impaired only when items were encoded under divided attention, indicating memory deficits that were secondary to deficits in the executive control. STBI patients could be differentiated into two distinct functional subgroups based on whether they favored a strategy of attending to the encoding or digit-monitoring task. The subgroup favoring the digit-monitoring task demonstrated deficits in the focused attention condition, and disproportionate memory deficits in the divided attention condition. In contrast, the subgroup favoring the encoding task demonstrated intact performance across all memory measures, regardless of attentional load, and despite remarkable similarity to the other STBI subgroup on demographic, neuropsychological, and acute injury severity measures. We discuss these outcome differences in terms of the relationship between strategy and executive control and highlight the need for more sensitive anatomical and behavioral measurement at both acute and chronic stages of injury.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) commonly report residual deficits in memory and attention that interfere with their everyday lives, often preventing them from returning to work or school at their pre-injury level [16], [39], [40], [43] and [63]. Indeed, a recent report found that both patients and their close relatives find poor memory to be the most troubling problem associated with the long-term (>6 years) outcome of TBI [45]. Yet, it is not uncommon for these subjective reports to contrast with intact performance on objective clinical measures of cognitive functions, particularly when acute injury is mild or moderate and/or medial temporal lobe structures are intact [6], [41] and [59]. The goal of the present study is to test one hypothesis that has emerged to account for this apparent paradox: that memory deficits are largely due to deficits in attention and/or executive control, and thus, may only be apparent when task demands on attention and its strategic allocation are sufficiently high [34], [49] and [60]. Dual-task conditions provide an experimental environment in which to test both attentional capacity and the ability to exert control over the allocation of attentional resources. Numerous behavioral and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that divided attention limits the likelihood that information will be processed to a deep, semantic level in temporal and inferior prefrontal corticies, and interferes with strategic organization of the material, a process putatively subserved by dorsolateral prefrontal regions [3], [12], [19] and [32]. Thus, we would expect that divided attention would interfere with episodic memory formation not only in TBI patients, but also in control subjects. However, encoding under divided attention is not only a function of the quantity of available resources, but also the ability to allocate these resources in an efficient, goal-directed manner [4], [50] and [51]. The finding that dual-task performance can faithfully reflect instructed trade-offs (i.e. primary-task emphasis, secondary-task emphasis, or equal emphasis) demonstrates that attention is a resource whose distribution is under control of a supervisory or executive control process [2] and [8]. Thus, performance on the primary (memory) task can only be properly evaluated when performance on the secondary task, and any potential strategic trade-offs that may occur between tasks, are taken into account. If executive control processes were impaired by TBI, we would expect the degree of impairment when information is encoded under divided attention to be disproportionate to that of control subjects, and to be unaccounted for by performance on either task alone [5]. Deficits in executive control have been associated with damage to the prefrontal cortex, but may also occur as the result of damaged connections between the prefrontal cortex and posterior regions [11] and [38]. Fronto-temporal damage and diffuse axonal shearing, particularly of longitudinal connections, are common sequelae of the typical deceleration/acceleration injury associated with TBI [25]. In particular, orbitofrontal and anterior temporal regions are vulnerable to TBI because of the jagged internal geometry of the skull around the orbits and cranial nerve processes, although lesions occasionally occur more superiorly [34]. Thus, the hypothesis that executive control deficits form a core feature of TBI appears to fit with the neurophysiological profile of the typical TBI patient. It is therefore surprising that many behavioral studies have failed to provide unambiguous support for a specific deficit in executive control (for review see [49]). For example, when required to simultaneously perform a dot-counting task while engaged in a driving simulator, no disproportionate deficits were found in subacute patients (<30 days post-injury) when slowing on the single-tasks was taken into account [40] and [64]. Vilkki et al. [65] also failed to find dual-task deficits in either subacute patients or patients with focal frontal lesions. Deficits were found, however, in acute patients, suggesting that time-since-injury influenced performance. Age at the time of injury may also be a factor. Stablum et al. [54] did not find dual-task deficits in MTBI patients under 30 years old, but did find deficits in older patients. Finally, the nature of the secondary task and the degree to which it taxes both attentional resources and executive control is likely to influence performance. Hartman et al. [18] found that conversation with the experimenter caused disproportionate impairment in STBI patients on a visual-motor tracking task, but a digit-span task did not. In contrast, McDowell et al. [36] found that concurrent performance of a digit-span task was sufficient to disrupt simple reaction time in patients with either subacute or acute severe TBI. Data on the specific effects of dual-tasks on long-term memory in TBI patients is sparser and even less supportive of a clear deficit in attention and/or executive function. To our knowledge, the only studies that have investigated the effects of divided attention on episodic memory performance in chronic patients have used severe TBI patients. In these studies, patients were equally impaired in both focused and divided attention conditions, rather than disproportionately impaired in the divided attention condition, as would be predicted by a specific deficit in executive control [48] and [66]. The effects of divided attention on the memory performance of patients with mild and moderate-to-severe TBI also have not been compared directly. Whereas patients with severe TBI may sustain chronic deficits in their ability to encode new information into episodic memory regardless of attentional load, mild TBI may result in a memory deficit that is only revealed under the greater attentional demands of dual-task processing. In addition, these previous studies only assessed performance on retrieval tasks that provided some type of environmental support (i.e. cued recall, recognition). Thus, far, the effects of divided attention in TBI have not been evaluated on retrieval tasks that are specifically associated with effortful processing and frontal-lobe function, such as tests of free recall and source memory [67]. Even in normal adults, divided attention at encoding affects source memory to a greater extent than item memory [62]. In the present study we investigate the extent to which attention and/or executive control resulting from TBI influence episodic memory performance. Specifically, we hypothesize that in the case of mild TBI, deficits in the executive control of attention interfere with the ability of intact memory processes to function optimally, whereas in moderate-to-severe TBI, deficits in executive control only further exacerbate primary memory deficits. Indeed, for patients with more severe TBI, a basic impairment in episodic encoding, rather than executive control of attentional resources, may constitute the core deficit. To address these hypotheses, we measured episodic memory in a paradigm where we carefully controlled and/or manipulated the patient, stimulus, and test factors over which attention and executive control processes would likely vary. Specifically, patients with mild or moderate-to-severe TBI were compared to age, education, and SES matched controls on memory for item (object) and context (scene) associations under conditions of focused or divided attention at encoding. We explored the extent to which factors in the acute stage of TBI predicted cognitive outcome by subdividing patients for analysis in two ways. First, patients were divided a priori according to differences in injury severity (i.e. Glasgow Coma Score (GCS), loss of consciousness (LOC), and post-traumatic amnesia (PTA)) and evaluated in terms of behavioral differences. Then patients were subdivided a posteriori according to differences in behavioral performance in the dual-task condition (i.e. apparent differences in strategic trade-offs between primary and secondary tasks) and evaluated in terms of neurobehavioral and demographic differences. With regard to stimuli, we manipulated the extent to which objects were semantically congruent with their associated scene. Given that attention appears to be a necessary condition for deep, elaborative processing, deficits resulting from reduced resources might be exacerbated when the association between object and scene is incongruous (e.g. kitchen–clown). For incongruous associations, lack of pre-existing semantic associations may make the integration of object and scene more effortful, and thus, more susceptible to disruption from increased attentional load. Similarly, we predicted that impaired attention would produce greater deficits on memory tests that required greater integration of source/context with item information, such as tests of free and source-cued recall, as is often found in patients with dorsolateral prefrontal damage (e.g. [14], [21], [31], [56] and [67]). Therefore, we assessed memory using free recall, scene-cued recall, object recognition and object–scene recognition tests, and measured not only total correct responses, but also intrusion errors and strategic organization. To preview our results, although we did not find straightforward interactions between group (TBI versus controls) and attention condition (focused versus divided) for most memory measures, exploratory post hoc comparisons and measures of divided attention “cost” did provide some evidence that the TBI patients experienced greater declines in memory performance under divided attention than control subjects, garnering some support for the hypothesis that deficits in the executive control of attention contribute, at least in part, to memory problems in TBI. Yet, the magnitude of these impairments was relatively uninfluenced by the stimulus and test manipulations that we hypothesized would be diagnostic of reduced attentional resources. One explanation for this somewhat unexpected pattern of results is that TBI influenced the ability to strategically allocate resources across multiple tasks more than the absolute amount of available resources. The importance of considering allocation strategy in interpreting TBI memory performance was even more evident when we scrutinized individual differences in trade-offs between the encoding and digit-monitoring task. In this a posteriori analysis, we found that moderate-to-severe TBI patients could be characterized according to whether they appeared to bias attention toward encoding or digit-monitoring tasks in the dual-task condition. Those patients who biased attention toward the digit-monitoring task demonstrated a pattern of performance consistent with a primary deficit in memory encoding, such as deficits on both free recall and standardized neuropsychological memory performance tested under focused attention conditions, whereas patients who biased attention toward the encoding task performed with normal limits on all tasks. Although the groups resulting from this subdivision were small, and therefore caution must be exercised in interpreting the results, we use these findings to emphasize the importance of considering how TBI patients strategically allocate attention in interpreting their performance in multi-tasking situations.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

Using sensitive tests of effortful episodic encoding and retrieval we were able to discern executive control deficits in patients with both mild and moderate-to-severe TBI. Yet, had TBI groups been subdivided only on the basis of acute severity (i.e. a priori), and outcome been measured only according to performance on focused attention and/or standardized neuropsychological tests of attention and memory, no significant differences between controls and either MTBI or STBI(all) groups would have emerged. Indeed, regardless of injury severity, psychometric test performance appeared largely intact in these patients, indicating clinical evidence of relatively “good” recovery of function [59]. Rather impairments were most evident in the TBI patients when information was encoded under divided attention—conditions that may more realistically simulate the multi-tasking demands associated with the activities that these patients find most challenging in everyday life. The demands of dual-task performance also revealed functional heterogeneity within the STBI group. When subdivided according to performance trade-offs, different patterns of memory performance emerged not only on all experimental measures, but also on standardized memory tests that were given 1–1.5 years prior to current testing. Thus, our experimental measures revealed persistent and pervasive memory and executive control deficits in the STBI(DB) subgroup, rather than deficits specific only to our tasks. Admittedly, these subgroups were small and their evaluation was necessarily post hoc, however if we had not assessed the relationship between encoding and digit-monitoring tasks, interesting strategy differences would have remained obscured and the pattern of results would have been weaker and more ambiguous. Although these results need to be replicated by future studies that directly manipulate strategic resource allocation, our current findings are valuable in suggesting that the variability of results from previous dual-task studies may be a product of uncontrolled variability in patient strategy. Indeed, by combining direct manipulation of task emphasis with more precise neurobehavioral and neurophysiological assessment at the time of acute injury, it may be possible to determine what combination of organic and behavioral factors predict these strategy differences. More precise characterization of a patient’s response to dual-task challenge also may have important consequences for choices of rehabilitative treatment. For example, Alderman [1] found that the ability to appropriately allocate resources under dual-task conditions better discriminated whether patients would benefit from a behavioral program of rehabilitation than measures of general intelligence, memory, or frontal-lobe function. Although in the present study, heterogeneity of strategy could only be observed in the group effect, manipulation of task emphasis may be able to reveal strategy differences on an individual basis, where it could provide important information regarding the direction and success of an individual’s rehabilitation program.