دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 34730
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

اعتبار خویشتن داری غذایی در میان دختران 5 تا 9 ساله

عنوان انگلیسی
Validity of dietary restraint among 5- to 9-year old girls
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
34730 2004 7 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Appetite, Volume 42, Issue 3, June 2004, Pages 241–247

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
- خویشتن داری غذایی - اعتبار - فرزندان -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Dietary restraint; Validity; Children.
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  اعتبار خویشتن داری غذایی در میان دختران  5 تا 9 ساله

چکیده انگلیسی

Dietary restraint is defined as the cognitive tendency to restrict intake and is often accompanied by the breakdown of restraint, referred to as disinhibition, leading to overeating and overweight in adults. Given recent evidence suggesting that dietary restraint and disinhibition are emerging as early as middle childhood, this study examined the validity of the restraint construct as measured in girls between the ages of 5 and 9. Dietary restraint was assessed longitudinally by questionnaire and validity was established by correlating restraint with measures previously reported to be related to restraint including weight concerns, body esteem, self-reported dieting, and measures of dietary intake when girls were ages 5, 7, and 9. Participants were 153 girls from predominately middle class, and exclusively non-Hispanic white families living in central Pennsylvania. Correlational data were used to assess relationships between dietary restraint and weight concerns, body esteem, dietary intake, and dieting. Results from this study indicate that there is evidence for the validity of the dietary restraint construct among girls by age 9. Specifically, dietary restraint was highly and positively associated with body mass index, weight concerns and body dissatisfaction and negatively correlated with dietary intake, findings similar to those reported in the adult literature.

مقدمه انگلیسی

The construct of dietary restraint was originally defined as the cognitive tendency to restrict intake as a means of maintaining or losing body weight (Herman & Polivy, 1975), and among adolescents and adults, tends to be highly related to dieting behavior, body dissatisfaction, and weight concerns (Killen et al., 1996, Polivy and Herman, 1985, Stice and Whitenton, 2002 and Wardle, 1987). Initially, dietary restraint was believed to emerge during adolescence as a consequence of the normative weight gain associated with puberty (Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, & Rodin, 1986). This weight gain was hypothesized to cause girls to become more dissatisfied with their bodies and engage in more frequent and extreme weight control measures (Attie and Brooks-Gunn, 1989 and Levine, 1987). However, there is now evidence that dietary restraint and disinhibition are emerging even earlier; and it appears that maladaptive eating attitudes and behaviors may begin to emerge in girls as early as age 5 (Carper et al., 2000 and Cutting et al., 1999). Understanding how and when dietary restraint is manifest in young girls is critical because dietary restraint may place children at risk for subsequent disordered eating (Killen et al., 1994, Marchi and Cohen, 1990, Stice, 2001 and Stice and Whitenton, 2002). Because studies assessing the construct of dietary restraint during childhood are limited, the aim of this research is to explore the validity of dietary restraint, as measured in girls between the ages of 5 and 9. When dietary restraint was first introduced as a construct, the terms ‘restraint’ and ‘dieting’ were often used interchangeably (Herman and Polivy, 1984 and Polivy and Herman, 1985) and both terms implied a focus on self-imposed caloric deprivation. More recently, distinctions between restraint and dieting have been made with restraint reflecting the intent to restrict intake, while dieting represents the active use of weight control strategies to restrict intake. This distinction implies that at any given time, not all restrained eaters are currently dieting (Lowe, Foster, Kerzhnerman, Swain, & Waddes, 2001). However, Van Strien and colleagues state that a restraint scale is only valid “if a score reflects the degree to which an individual eats less then he or she actually would like to eat.” (Van Strien, Fritjers, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) Therefore, consistent with the majority of literature and for purposes of this research the terms ‘restrained eater’ and ‘dieter’ will be used interchangeably (Herman & Polivy, 1984). In children, understanding how the constructs of dietary restraint and dieting are interpreted can be particularly problematic. For example, it seems likely that reports indicating that as many as 60% of adolescent girls are currently dieting (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2000) may overestimate dieting prevalence, because self-reported dieting may not indicate actual reduction in intake but may reflect the intent to lose weight. A study by Neumark-Sztainer and Story (1998) indicated that adolescents viewed dieting as an umbrella term for many behaviors aimed at weight control, including physical activity. In research evaluating the degree to which adolescents report and adopt weight control behaviors, 44% of adolescent females reported dieting on the previous day yet only 8.6% of the food records reflected dieting (Nichter, Ritenbaugh, Nichter, Vuckovic, & Aickin, 1995). Similarly, Field, Wolf, Herzog, Cheung, and Colditz (1993) found that self-reported frequent dieting in preadolescence and young adolescent girls was more indicative of extreme concern with weight than of actual restriction of reported intake. However, in this same study, Field and colleagues also found that by late adolescence, dieters were reporting significantly less energy intake than their non-dieting peers, but this may simply reflect greater under-reporting by restrained eaters as often seen in the adult literature. To investigate the validity of the restraint construct associations between restraint and measures that are related to restraint among children, adolescents, and adults were examined, including: body dissatisfaction, weight concerns, and reported dieting practices. For example in a study of 9-year-olds, Hill and colleagues validated children's reports of dietary restraint by noting a significant relationship between restraint and body shape dissatisfaction (Hill, Draper, & Stack, 1994). Among adults and adolescents, numerous studies have also identified links between body dissatisfaction and restraint (Cooley and Toray, 2001, Stice, 2001, Stice et al., 1998 and Wertheim et al., 2001) dieting practices and restraint (Cooley and Toray, 2001, Killen et al., 1996, Killen et al., 1994, Leon et al., 1999 and Patton et al., 1990) and weight concerns and restraint (Heatherton et al., 1995, Killen et al., 1994 and Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2002). Evidence for the validity of restraint among young girls would be reflected in a similar pattern of relationships among restraint, weight concerns, body dissatisfaction, and dieting practices as previously observed among these constructs in women and adolescent girls, that is, restraint should be positively related to body dissatisfaction, weight concerns, reports of dieting attempts and practices. While the data in children are limited, studies relating dietary restraint to various measures of food intake have revealed modest predictive validity. For example, Hill and colleagues (1994) found that in a sample of 35 9-year old school children, high levels of restraint were inversely related to reported energy intake. However, similar to adult samples (Bingham et al., 1995, Black et al., 1995 and MacDiarmid and Blundell, 1997), high levels of restraint in children may be related to under-reporting, but not to actual restrictions in intake, with restrained individuals being more likely to under-report their intake. The reasons for under-reporting in highly restrained adults are unknown, however, many speculate that under-reporting may be a result of self-deception, rather than a means of deception, or that those who intend to diet may actually make attempts at dieting during the study period; regardless of the reasons for under-reporting, it is pervasive; one recent study estimated the prevalence of under-reporting between 18 and 31% (MacDiarmid & Blundell, 1998). It is also possible that girls are reporting their intentions to restrict intake rather than their actual intakes. In the present study, the availability of weighed food intake measures should shed light on the extent to which relationships between restraint and reported intake are attributable to reporting bias or whether they reflect actual relationships between restraint and intake.