دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 35223
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

تاثیر روان رنجوری بر روی گزارش دهی همزمان علائم: روش مدل سازی چند سطحی

عنوان انگلیسی
The influence of neuroticism on concurrent symptom reporting: A multilevel modelling approach
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
35223 2006 12 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Personality and Individual Differences, Volume 41, Issue 3, August 2006, Pages 549–560

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
روان رنجوری - گزارش دهی همزمان علائم - الگوی روزانه - تجزیه و تحلیل چند سطحی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Neuroticism; Concurrent symptom reporting; Daily pattern; Multilevel analysis
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  تاثیر روان رنجوری بر روی گزارش دهی همزمان علائم: روش مدل سازی چند سطحی

چکیده انگلیسی

The current study explored the influence of neuroticism on the diurnal pattern of symptom reporting. Participants were 548 individuals from 169 families. Using a computer-assisted self-monitoring procedure, individuals reported their concurrent somatic symptoms six times per day for seven consecutive days. Neuroticism was assessed separately in a follow-up questionnaire. We used a generalized linear models approach for multilevel-analysis, and analysed a four level model with observations within days, days within individuals, and individuals within families. Results show no main effect for neuroticism, but an interaction of neuroticism with time of the observation. Individuals average in neuroticism show a curvilinear pattern with increased symptom reporting in the morning and in the evening, and less during the day. Individuals with low levels of neuroticism show a similar but more attenuated pattern. Individuals high in neuroticism, however, show a constant level of symptom reporting throughout the day, with no decrease in the middle of the day. These findings suggest that neuroticism results not so much in exaggerated symptom reporting but in a different diurnal pattern of concurrent symptom reporting.

مقدمه انگلیسی

For the last 20 years, neuroticism and negative affectivity have been the most consistent predictors of symptom reporting especially in retrospective questionnaires (e.g. Costa and McCrae, 1980, Vassend and Skrondal, 1999, Watson and Pennebaker, 1989 and Williams and Wiebe, 2000). In the last decade, a much weaker association has also been shown with concurrent symptom reports (Cohen et al., 1995, Fahrenberg et al., 1999, Feldman et al., 1999, Kolk et al., 2003, Larsen, 1992, Neitzert et al., 1997 and Williams et al., 2002). However, only rarely have the concurrent measures been analysed without aggregating. The current study was designed to examine the influence of neuroticism on concurrent symptom reporting with multiple measures of symptom reporting per day. This procedure allowed an examination of differences between individuals high and low in neuroticism not only on their average level of symptom reporting but in their daily pattern. 1.1. Concurrent symptom reporting and its association with neuroticism Neuroticism is a stable personality characteristic associated with the experience of negative emotions. Often, individuals high in neuroticism are very ruminative, especially regarding their health and well-being. Neuroticism is often assessed as emotional instability (e.g. Freiburg Personality Inventory, Fahrenberg, Hampel, & Selg, 2001) or negative affectivity (e.g. the trait measure of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jabobs, 1983). Because in the published literature the term neuroticism is still widely used as a superordinate concept of emotional stability and negative affectivity, in the current paper the term neuroticism will be used. In contrast to retrospective studies, prospective studies of symptom reporting show a smaller or no association with neuroticism. A higher report of somatic symptoms is usually found in reports assessed at the end of the day (diary approach; Cohen et al., 1995, Feldman et al., 1999 and Larsen, 1992). Studies using an experience or time sampling approach on the other hand, find an association between emotional state and symptom reporting but no (Brown & Moskowitz, 1997), or only a very small association with neuroticism (Fahrenberg et al., 1999 and Williams et al., 2002). In diary studies, individuals usually report their symptoms for the past 24 hours. Even if retrospection is abridged in this approach, it is still present to a certain degree. The phenomenon that even in short retrospection, experiences are recalled as worse than they really were, was called retrospection effect (Fahrenberg et al., 1999 and Kaeppler and Rieder, 2001). These findings suggest that neuroticism only has an effect on health measures if these are recalled from memory; in contrast, emotional states may affect the experience of a somatic event. Nonetheless, high and low neurotic individuals may differ in their somatic experience because of a different prevalence of positive and negative emotional states. While individuals’ average in neuroticism have an up and down in their emotions during the day (Wilhelm, 2001), high scoring neurotics may experience negative states more frequently, which increases attention to the body, makes them more ruminative about somatic experiences, and lets them discover somatic symptoms. Differing daily patterns in neurotics have been found for various psychological characteristics such as emotional states and performance indices (Fahrenberg et al., 1999, Kaeppler and Rieder, 2001 and Wilhelm, 2001), and may also be found in symptom reporting. 1.2. Neuroticism and diurnal patterns Circadian rhythmicity and its associations with personality characteristics has often been measured using the morningness–eveningness questionnaire (MEQ, Horne & Östberg, 1976). While some studies did not find differences for neuroticism (e.g. Adan, 1992 and Wilson, 1990), others report an association between neuroticism and eveningness (Adan and Almirall, 1990, Mecacci and Rocchetti, 1998 and Neubauer, 1992), e.g. a higher performance level in the evening, and a peak in body temperature later in the day in high scoring neurotics. These studies, however, rely on a one-time self-evaluation questionnaire to assess variation in diurnal patterns. Studies that compared neuroticism with measures assessed at different times throughout the day are rare. Lund (1974) found high internal desynchronization of activity and body temperature in individuals scoring high in neuroticism after they have been isolated from any time cues for 20 to 30 days. In a more recent study, high scoring neurotics did not show a clear circadian rhythm of positive emotions, and their emotional state was not related to body temperature (Murray, Allen, Trinder, & Burgess, 2002). The circadian rhythm of body temperature itself was attenuated in highly neurotic individuals. Using an experience sampling method, Rusting and Larsen (1998) found an association between neuroticism and depressive symptomatology in the evening. The preliminary conclusion from these studies can be summarized as a generally weakened circadian rhythmicity in individuals high in neuroticism with the possibility of a worsening of the emotional state in the evening. This may also be the case for the association between neuroticism and symptom reporting. Symptom reporting has been associated with a time pattern of increased complaints in the morning and evening, and decreased complaints during the middle of the day (Goebel and Cordes, 1990 and Michel, in press). Whereas this pattern may be true for individuals’ average in neuroticism, highly neurotic individuals might differ in their circadian pattern. This study thus investigated if (a) there is a difference in the average probability to report symptoms during the day and (b) there is a difference in the diurnal pattern of symptom reporting between individuals high and low in neuroticism. We expected no difference in the average probability but a differing diurnal pattern. (c) In addition, the structure of the variability in symptom reporting was investigated.