دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 35414
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

روان رنجوری، بازدارنده رفتاری، و واکنش به استقراء گسسته خلق و خوی منفی

عنوان انگلیسی
Neuroticism, BIS, and reactivity to discrete negative mood inductions
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
35414 2013 6 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Personality and Individual Differences, Volume 54, Issue 2, January 2013, Pages 208–213

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
روان رنجوری - سیستم بازداری رفتاری - خلق و خوی - احساسات - واکنش -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Neuroticism; Behavioral inhibition system; Mood; Emotion; Reactivity
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  روان رنجوری، بازدارنده رفتاری، و واکنش به استقراء گسسته خلق و خوی منفی

چکیده انگلیسی

Research has established relationships between the personality dimensions of neuroticism and BIS and broad negative emotional reactivity. However, few researchers have examined the relationships among neuroticism, BIS, and discrete negative emotional reactivities. The present study examined whether individuals scoring high on neuroticism and BIS were more reactive across four discrete negative mood inductions, relative to those scoring low on these traits. Participants (n = 166) completed personality questionnaires, measures of current mood, viewed a specific mood-inducing film clip (sadness, anger, fear or disgust) and then reported their moods a second time. Results revealed that neuroticism/BIS was associated with high reactivity to the fear and sadness inductions. Neuroticism/BIS did not predict anger or disgust reactivity, but neuroticism/BIS and extraversion/BAS interacted in predicting anger. Although further research is needed to extend generalizability, it appears that neuroticism and BIS predict negative emotional reactivity broadly, but not ubiquitously.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Individuals clearly differ in how much they experience pleasant and unpleasant emotions and these differences are well predicted by the broad traits of extraversion and neuroticism. That is, people high in extraversion report more pleasant emotional experience, and people high in neuroticism report more unpleasant emotional experience (Costa & McCrae, 1980). Moreover, at least some of the variation in experience follows from differences in reactivity to positive and negative stimuli in the environment. As opposed to tonic mean level differences (‘set points’), people high in extraversion and neuroticism react to positive and negative emotional events (respectively) with more intense emotions compared to people scoring low on these traits (e.g., Larsen & Ketelaar, 1989). Although many studies have demonstrated these effects, they typically assess emotional experience as broad dimensions of positive and negative affect. In this study, we examine neuroticism as a predictor of more discrete emotional reactivities. That is, we ask the question, does neuroticism predict responses to fear, sadness, disgust, and anger stimuli equally well? Psychologists have taken two broad approaches to understanding emotion states. The dimensional approach has focused on the structure of emotions, primarily using statistical tools like factor analysis. This approach often yields two independent, orthogonal dimensions such as pleasantness and arousal ( Larsen & Diener, 1992), or activated pleasant ‘positive affect’ (PA) and activated unpleasant ‘negative affect’ (NA; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). This approach uses experiential similarity or co-occurrence to organize affects, often relying on self-reported experience ( Larsen & Diener, 1992), though functional accounts have also been proposed (e.g., Cacioppo & Berntson, 1999). In contrast, the categorical approach has avoided statistical methods of data reduction, and instead relied more on theory and observation to discern which emotions are primary. Advocates of this approach, view basic (or discrete) emotions as irreducible, distinct states that rarely co-occur and are experientially unique (e.g., Izard, 1977, Plutchik, 1980 and Tomkins, 1984). Psychologists do not always agree on the criteria that make an emotion primary, and have considered things such as facial expressions, peripheral physiological signatures, neural substrates, action tendencies, appraisal patterns, etc. ( Ortony & Turner, 1990). Nonetheless, there appears to be consensus that sadness, fear, anger and disgust generally qualify as primary discrete emotions. Although there are exceptions, personality research has typically taken a dimensional approach to emotions. For example, the dimensions of positive affect and negative affect are consistently related to the personality dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism, respectively. With correlational methods, negative affect and neuroticism have been strongly linked in both brief studies of current mood and in longitudinal studies of average daily moods over time (Costa and McCrae, 1980, Emmons and Diener, 1985, Watson et al., 1999 and Zelenski and Larsen, 1999). More experimental methods have found that people high in neuroticism are particularly susceptible to negative mood inductions. That is, even when stimuli are carefully controlled in the laboratory, neuroticism predicts increased negative affect (e.g., Gomez et al., 2000, Gross et al., 1998, Larsen and Ketelaar, 1989, Larsen and Ketelaar, 1991 and Zelenski and Larsen, 1999), a finding that extends to physiological indicators (Canli et al., 2001 and Norris et al., 2007). Because the lab situation is controlled, we can infer that internal, temperamental differences explain these differences in emotional experience (as opposed to situational selection effects). These temperamental differences are often understood using Gray’s (1981) seminal Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. Gray proposed that people differ from each other in their relative sensitivity to reward and punishment cues in the environment. Three systems underlie these differences; the behavioral activation system (BAS) is responsive to cues of reward and avoidance of punishment, the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) is responsive to cues of punishment and frustrative non-reward, and the fight or fight system (FFS) is a threat response system that mediates responses to unconditioned aversive stimuli, resulting in escape (flight) or defensive aggression (fight). Furthermore, differences in BAS and BIS strength may underlie the traits of extraversion and neuroticism, respectively, such that BAS creates greater positive (reward) reactivity among those scoring higher on extraversion and BIS creates greater negative (punishment) reactivity among those scoring higher on neuroticism (Larsen and Ketelaar, 1989 and Zelenski and Larsen, 1999). Revisions to Gray’s theory (Gray & McNaughton, 2000) now parse the functions of the ‘BIS’ and ‘FFS’ a bit differently. The new FFFS (Fight, Flight or Freezing System) is now deemed responsible for some previous BIS functions, and more clearly linked to negative emotional reactions. The BIS is now viewed more as a conflict monitor or comparator that detects conflict between the BAS and new FFFS (e.g., in environments with both rewarding and punishing features; see review by Smillie, Pickering, & Jackson, 2006). This further emphasizes how behavior/experience often depends on both punishment and reward systems, particularly when situations are ambiguous or contain clear conflict between incentives and punishments (i.e., the joint subsystems hypothesis; Corr, 2002). Despite this re-organization, popular assessment tools (e.g., Carver & White, 1994) still assess approach and avoidance tendencies and label these propensities as BAS and BIS more in line with Gray’s original conception. At the level of psychological experience, these measures of broad sensitivities to reward and punishment are still useful in testing the motivational and emotional associates of extraversion and neuroticism even if they fail to capture some nuances of the revised RST. Although both trait neuroticism and BIS have been linked to negative emotional reactivity, previous research has rarely considered discrete negative emotions. That is, the broad trait of neuroticism describes a propensity for general unpleasant experience, yet predictions of reactivity drawn from Gray’s theory need not be so broad. Researchers have assumed that neuroticism promotes reactivity for the many unpleasant emotions (fear, sadness, anger, disgust, etc.) that tend to conglomerate in the broad negative affect dimension, and they have not taken care in isolating the potentially distinct reactivities. The fact that unpleasant affects cohere in a single factor at the trait level need not imply that a single process is responsible. For example, a temperamental susceptibility to fear might cause a person to make choices that ultimately create sadness (e.g., avoiding social situations). In the lab, this person might show high fear reactivity, but normal sadness reactivity, because their high trait sadness is due to lifestyle choices rather than temperamental sensitivity per se. Although not always explicitly stated in the theory, we can use Gray’s RST to make some more specific predictions. In a chapter addressing basic emotions, Gray (1994) identified anxiety as resulting from BIS and rage and panic as resulting from FFS, but this predated his theoretical revisions to these systems. Nonetheless, the new FFFS, or the original BIS, seems clearly linked to fear as a primary output of the avoidance system. In addition, disgust includes elements of avoidance (e.g., moving away from bad smells), and thus disgust reactivity might be higher in people with a strong punishment system. Old and new versions of Gray’s theory both suggest that anger (‘fight’) is related to the FFFS; however, this additional element is not included in our RST assessment tool, and thus anger reactivity will probably not correlate with our measure of original BIS functioning. Gray’s theory does not provide a clear rationale to predict laboratory-based sadness reactivity among high BIS people – yet it is not a great jump to view sadness as stemming from sensitivity to punishment. For instance, past reactivity studies appear to have induced sadness through punishment-like stimuli, e.g., by using narratives about the death of a friend or failure feedback (Larsen and Ketelaar, 1989 and Larsen and Ketelaar, 1991). BAS may also play a role in negative emotional reactivity. Some have linked both anger and sadness to approach, rather than avoidance, systems (e.g., Harmon-Jones, 2003 and Plutchik, 1980). For instance, Carver (2004) considered the BAS as underlying the approach behaviors involved with fighting and the frustration (anger) resulting from blocked rewards. Deficits in approach behavior associated with sadness may also result from a weak BAS. Although BIS clearly predicts depression, low BAS is also implicated (Hundt et al., 2007 and Kasch et al., 2002). In addition, Corr (2002) found that high BIS strengthened reactions to fearful slides, but this effect was even stronger among low impulsivity participants (that is, the combination of high BIS, low BAS). Thus, we examine BAS as an additional contributor to negative reactivities.