دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 32215
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

ارزیابی خودشیفتگی در تحقیقات اجتماعی ـ شخصیت: آیا ارتباط بین خودشیفتگی و نتیجه سلامت روانی از اعتماد به نفس حاصل می شود؟

عنوان انگلیسی
Narcissism assessment in social–personality research: Does the association between narcissism and psychological health result from a confound with self-esteem?
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
32215 2010 13 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Journal of Research in Personality, Volume 44, Issue 4, August 2010, Pages 453–465

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
- () خودشیفتگی - عزت نفس - سلامت روان - پرسشنامه شخصیت خودشیفتگی () - اختلال شخصیت خودشیفته - خودشیفتگی پاتولوژیک - خودشیفتگی عادی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Narcissism; Self-esteem; Psychological health; Narcissistic personality inventory (NPI); Narcissistic personality disorder; Pathological narcissism; Normal narcissism
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  ارزیابی خودشیفتگی در تحقیقات اجتماعی ـ شخصیت: آیا ارتباط بین خودشیفتگی و نتیجه سلامت روانی از اعتماد به نفس حاصل می شود؟

چکیده انگلیسی

Influential social and personality psychology research indicates that narcissism is related to psychological health. Such inferences are open to question, however, because they nearly all rely on the same self-report instrument—the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin and Hall, 1979 and Raskin and Hall, 1981)—to operationalize and measure narcissism. This is problematic because numerous NPI items do not appear to correspond to common definitions or manifestations of narcissism, and may instead be indicative of self-esteem. Two studies demonstrate that the NPI’s confound with self-esteem accounts for the purported relationship between narcissism and psychological health. This suggests that inferences about narcissism and psychological health may need to be reinterpreted. Results also highlight the need for measures that correspond more directly to core components of narcissism.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Imagine hearing about two people who are both described as assertive and confident. Both also prefer leadership roles and enjoy receiving compliments. However, beyond these apparent similarities, the two are quite different from each other. The first is consistently full of bravado to overcompensate for core insecurities, insatiably in need of other people’s admiration (e.g., Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001), and unreasonably pushy, overbearing, and demanding. You probably would not be surprised to hear this person referred to as a narcissist (e.g., American Psychiatric Association, 2000). In contrast, the second person is “happier than most, less socially anxious, … less depressed, … [and] higher [in] self-esteem” (see W. K. Campbell, 2001, p. 214). Is it possible that this normatively happy and healthy person is a narcissist as well? According to some perspectives in social and personality psychology research (e.g., Campbell, 2001 and Miller and Campbell, 2008), the answer may be “yes.” In fact, the psychologically healthy characteristics enumerated above are a partial description of a narcissist from one social–personality point of view. The narcissist with these healthy traits lies at the center of a crucial question about the limits of the theoretical and operational definitions of narcissism. To what extent should high self-esteem, and normative traits related to it such as assertiveness and confidence, play a role in defining narcissism? The divergence between normative social–personality perspectives on this question, and clinical perspectives, which generally emphasize narcissists’ low or fragile self-esteem (e.g., Association, 2000, Kernberg, 1975, Kohut, 1971 and Kohut, 1977), has made it increasingly difficult to reconcile social–personality and clinical narcissism research (Cain et al., 2008, Miller and Campbell, 2008, Pincus and Lukowitsky, 2010 and Pincus et al., 2009). Moreover, and most germane to this article, disagreement over whether self-esteem and other normative traits should be considered narcissistic has created confusion about whether psychological health is truly a potential characteristic of people who are narcissistic. Although it is clear that many social–personality psychologists include high self-esteem in their conceptions of narcissism whereas clinical psychologists generally do not (Fossati et al., 2005, Miller and Campbell, 2008, Pincus and Lukowitsky, 2010 and Pincus et al., 2009), it is unlikely that this reflects fundamental theoretical differences between the two camps. Instead, we posit that the inclusion of high self-esteem and other normative characteristics in social–personality definitions of narcissism results primarily from the way narcissism is traditionally operationalized. The self-report scale that is used to measure narcissism in nearly all social–personality research ( Cain et al., 2008, Mullins and Kopelman, 1988 and Pincus et al., 2009) is the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin and Hall, 1979 and Raskin and Hall, 1981). The NPI contains items that appear to be confounded with self-esteem, a characteristic that they may measure at least as effectively as they measure narcissism. As a result, the NPI’s correlates include not only the negative psychological states and outcomes that might be shared with typical clinical definitions of narcissism (e.g., aggression, Bushman & Baumeister, 1998; reduced romantic commitment, W. K. Campbell & Foster, 2002; pathological gambling, Lakey, Rose, Campbell, & Goodie, 2008), but also the positive psychological states that are more likely to be associated with high self-esteem, such as those noted in the description of the happy, healthy narcissist in example two. Self-esteem is the normative characteristic with which the NPI appears to share its most robust empirical relationship (see Brown and Zeigler-Hill, 2004, Cain et al., 2008, Raskin et al., 1991a, Raskin et al., 1991b and Rhodewalt and Morf, 1995). One reason to closely examine the NPI’s positive link with self-esteem, and the possibility that this link is largely a byproduct of confounded items contained within the NPI itself, is that there is accumulating evidence that the connection between the NPI and psychological health is contingent on the NPI’s relationship with self-esteem. This is the case, for example, in research by Rose (2002), who reported that narcissism was related to multiple measures of happiness, and by Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg, Kumashiro, and Rusbult (2004), who reported that narcissism was related to numerous indicators of psychological health, such as lower depression, sadness, anxiety, and neuroticism, as well as higher levels of personal and couple well-being. In both sets of studies, the relationship between the NPI and each of its healthy psychological correlates was fully mediated by self-esteem (see also Brown, Budzek, & Tamborski, 2009). This led Sedikides and colleagues to conclude that self-esteem is the “key component of narcissism” (p. 401) through which narcissism predicts positive psychological outcomes (see also Sinha & Krueger, 1998). In contrast, self-esteem does not appear to mediate the association between the NPI and the kinds of negative outcomes more traditionally associated with narcissism. For example, controlling for self-esteem did not negate the relationship between the NPI and reduced romantic commitment ( W. K. Campbell & Foster, 2002), pathological gambling ( Lakey et al., 2008), or the greedy exploitation of natural resources ( W. K. Campbell, Bush, Brunell, & Shelton, 2005). Taken together, it appears that a link between the NPI and psychological health (but not negative psychological outcomes) depends on a theory of narcissism that considers high self-esteem to be an inherent narcissistic trait (e.g., Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003). To justify the inclusion of self-esteem and other normative characteristics within a social–personality theory of narcissism, some have called for revisiting historic clinical theories that provide the grounds doing so (see Campbell, 2001 and Miller and Campbell, 2008). However, such an approach may be problematic for several reasons. First, it employs post hoc theoretical explanations for findings that might be more parsimoniously explained through an empirical critique of some of the NPI’s items. Further, the clinical theories that did include high self-esteem in their definition of narcissism were either too nebulous and overinclusive (e.g., Freud, 1931/1950) or too multifaceted (e.g., Kohut, 1971 and Kohut, 1977) to be operationalized properly using any single measure (see Clark and Watson, 1995 and Raskin and Terry, 1988). Perhaps most problematically, these historical clinical theories are not aligned with the theoretical foundation that was used to develop the NPI. The NPI was not developed to correspond with a normative definition of narcissism. Instead, the scale’s authors intended to provide a continuous, non-clinical measure (for use in normal populations) of the characteristics described by a specific clinical definition of narcissism, the diagnostic criteria for narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) in the then-current edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III, Association, 1980 and Raskin and Terry, 1988). Accordingly, the NPI’s authors developed items that they believed “sampled the domain of the narcissistic personality” (Raskin & Hall, 1979, p. 590) identified by the DSM-III criteria. These criteria included a grandiose sense of self-importance, preoccupation with fantasies of unlimited success, exhibitionism, indifference or rage, and disturbances in interpersonal relationships such as entitlement, exploitativeness, splitting, and lack of empathy. The NPI was based on the assumption that “abnormality is continuous with normality, [and thus] behaviors descriptive of the Narcissistic Personality Disorder are … extreme forms [of these narcissistic traits] which are manifested to a lesser extent in normal individuals” (Raskin & Hall, 1981, p. 159). In other words, the scale’s authors intended for the NPI to measure less extreme levels of characteristics associated with narcissism as described in the DSM-III (see Pincus and Lukowitsky, 2010 and Pincus et al., 2009). They did not intend to expand the definition of the construct based on normative clinical or social–personality theories. By referring to historic theories that included high self-esteem in their definition of narcissism to justify NPI-based findings about psychological health, critics (e.g., Campbell, 2001 and Miller and Campbell, 2008) implicitly suggest that the authors of the NPI got the measurement aspects of narcissism right, but got the theory on which they based their measure wrong. In contrast, we suggest that just the opposite may be the case; the NPI’s authors appear to have created a scale that is not entirely consistent with the theory on which it was based. In fact, the NPD section of the DSM-III states that narcissistic self-esteem is “often fragile” (p. 316), rather than that narcissists have high self-esteem. Using the DSM-III criteria as a theoretical “conceptual template” (Raskin & Terry, 1988, p. 892), it is easy to understand how items ranging from severe manifestations of narcissistic characteristics (e.g., “I insist on getting the respect that is due me;” “I know that I am good because everybody keeps telling me so”) to more moderate manifestations (e.g., “I am apt to show off if I get a chance;” “I am more capable than other people”) were included in the NPI. Unfortunately, other NPI items, such as “I am assertive” and “I see myself as a good leader” appear to fall outside of the NPI’s authors’ own DSM-III-based definition of narcissism. These more normative characteristics do not appear to lie on a clear continuum with those enumerated in the DSM-III. Rather, they most likely measure subjectively different constructs from the diagnostic criteria rather than less severe versions of the same constructs (see Pincus and Lukowitsky, 2010 and Pincus et al., 2009). A practical problem this causes is that the NPI’s most normative items are not specific to narcissism—a narcissist might be described by them (if only superficially), as in the first example presented earlier. But the content of these items also might not be indicative of narcissism. In other words, the items do not necessarily differentiate well between narcissists and non-narcissists. Even if narcissists often exhibit these characteristics (see Lynam and Widiger, 2001, Miller et al., 2009 and Samuel and Widiger, 2004), knowing that someone possesses these attributes does not necessarily help one discern how narcissistic that individual is, or even whether that individual is narcissistic at all. Further, such items may provide more information about whether a person has high self-esteem than about whether that person is narcissistic. And although such normative attributes are not measured by a majority of the NPI’s items, the internal consistency strategy originally used to develop the NPI resulted in statistical overrepresentation of its most normative items in aggregated scores ( Emmons, 1984, Kansi, 2003 and Raskin and Terry, 1988). As a result, aggregated NPI scores are poorly proportioned (see Haynes, Richard, & Kubany, 1995), causing them to follow the scale’s most normative items in correlating with variables indicative of psychological health. In sum, the conclusion that narcissism is associated with healthy psychological states and outcomes may depend largely on the effects of a number of items in the NPI that appear to be more closely related to self-esteem than to narcissism. To investigate this possibility, in Study 1 we examined whether the NPI’s overlap with self-esteem is attributable to a theoretically sound empirical relationship between the two constructs, or whether, as we hypothesize, it is the result of the inclusion of specific items in the NPI that are confounded with self-esteem. In Study 2, we explored whether these same NPI items account for the types of relationships between narcissism and psychological health that have been reported in the social–personality literature.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

Ultimately, the goal of any scale is “to measure one thing (i.e., the target construct)—and only this thing—as precisely as possible” (Clark & Watson, 1995, p. 315). To the extent that there is no definitive answer to the question of what constitutes narcissism and what does not (Millon, 1998 and Westen, 1990), this goal may be difficult to achieve. Most psychologists would agree that narcissists are entitled, exploitative, grandiose, self-absorbed, and so forth. Unfortunately, some inferences made in social–personality research are driven by the NPI’s relationship with characteristics that are at best tangential to these domains, such as self-esteem, assertiveness, and leadership motivations. This is evidenced by the fact that the item “I am assertive” was considered narcissistic by only one expert rater in Study 1. Our concern is that the NPI, and the construct it was developed to measure (i.e., narcissism), have developed a tail-wags-the-dog relationship in the social–personality literature. The NPI is currently the most frequent means for measuring narcissism in psychology research, and an aggregated score is the “most common usage” of the NPI (Miller & Campbell, 2008, p. 456). Because of this, it often appears that narcissism is no longer understood as a latent personality trait estimated by scores on the NPI. Instead, narcissism has become increasingly redefined as “that which the aggregated NPI measures.” Extensive use of the NPI has led to a situation in which social–personality and clinical narcissism research are increasingly difficult to integrate. It also has created the potential for reaching questionable conclusions about narcissism, particularly about its relationship to psychological health.