دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 36735
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

پیش بینی ترک تحصیل از آموزش های شناختی با عملکرد بر روی کار ضد پرش چشم

عنوان انگلیسی
Performance on the antisaccade task predicts dropout from cognitive training
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
36735 2015 7 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Intelligence, Volume 49, March–April 2015, Pages 25–31

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
مهار - آموزش شناختی - ترک تحصیل - ظرفیت حافظه کاری
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Inhibition; Cognitive training; Antisaccade; Dropout; Working memory capacity
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  پیش بینی ترک تحصیل از آموزش های شناختی با عملکرد بر روی کار ضد پرش چشم

چکیده انگلیسی

Cognitive training programs have gained immense popularity in recent years. Given that training regimes tend to be extremely demanding, and dropout rates are sometimes alarming, it is surprising how little we know about individual attributes that might predict dropout. We explored whether working memory capacity and inhibition, as measured in pre-test, might predict attrition from what turned out to be a dropout inducing cognitive training program. We found that overall working memory capacity did not predict dropout but that inhibition (poor performance in the Antisaccade task) did. Implications for the design of future training interventions are discussed.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Humans are adapted to accomplish long term goals through perseverance. As early as we can remember, we adhere to long term goals even when this entails considerable time and effort. School achievements (Hill & Tyson, 2009), learning to play an instrument (McPherson, 2000) and academic studies (Jeynes, 2005) are all effortful, time consuming activities associated with long term goals. In the present study, we explore whether executive functions, namely inhibition and working memory capacity (WMC), are associated with the ability to persevere on highly demanding tasks. To do so, we used a highly demanding cognitive training regimen that in hindsight, proved to be so demanding that it resulted in a considerable dropout. Capitalizing on this unexpected result and on the fact that inhibition and WMC were measured at pre-test, we tried to use these measures to predict dropout. Cognitive training programs have gained popularity for both scientific and commercial reasons (Klingberg, 2010, Morrison and Chein, 2011 and Shipstead et al., 2012). The possibility of enhancing ones' higher mental abilities such as working memory and fluid intelligence has profound implications on medical interventions, vocational and academic success and can teach us a great deal about brain plasticity. However, training programs also tend to be mentally tiresome and lengthy (for a review see von Bastian & Oberauer, 2013). This is partly because a positive dosage–outcome relation has been suggested in the literature, justifying the extended length of these interventions (Alloway et al., 2013 and Jaeggi et al., 2008). Surprisingly little is reported as to what may predict participant's attrition. One report found that participants who performed better on matrix reasoning tasks and were rated higher on a need for cognition scale (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) were more likely to complete a 20-sessions cognitive training program (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Shah & Jonides, 2014). Interestingly, in the same study as many as 29% of the trainees failed to complete the training program, suggesting that dropout is indeed a serious issue. Given these early findings, Executive functions may also be good candidates for predicting attrition. Executive functions have been associated with self-control (Barkley, 2001, Hofmann et al., 2012 and Miller and Cohen, 2001). Specifically, research has shown that executive functions contribute to the adherence to long term goals. For example, Hofmann, Friese and Roefs (2009) found that WMC, as measured by the Operation Span task (Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock, & Engle, 2005), and inhibition, as measured by the Stop Signal task (Logan, Schachar, & Tannock, 1997) each contributed uniquely to the ability to restrain eating behavior (see also Nederkoorn, Houben, Hofmann, Roefs, & Jansen, 2010). Why should inhibition and working memory capacity relate to perseverance? In regard to inhibition, Barkley (2001) suggests that the ability to inhibit a prepotent response in favor of a contextually favorable one is an adaptation to social challenges as is asking a person to keep training in a demanding and tiresome regimen. In order to fulfill it, the person should keep in mind the commitment (and perceived benefits) and suppress any urge to quit the commitment in favor of another immediately gratifying activity. In accordance with this notion, Berkman, Falk, and Lieberman (2011) showed that activation in brain regions associated with inhibition while performing a Go/No-Go task known to involve inhibition moderated the link between cigarette craving and subsequent smoking. A recent study also found that self-control (measured by the Behavioral Problems Index; see Zill, 1990) is positively correlated with education level, job complexity and income (Converse, Piccone & Tocci, 2014). As for specifically predicting dropout using individual differences in executive functions, it can only be inferred from other domains since up until now, no evidence from cognitive training studies exist. Evidence can be found in other domains: Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, De Beurs, and Van Den Brink (2008) reported that relapse of pathological gamblers was predicted by disinhibition, as measured by the Stop Signal task (Logan & Cowan, 1984), and relapse after one week of abstinence from smoking was more likely to occur in smokers less accurate in the Antisaccade task (Powell, Dawkins, West, Powell, & Pickering, 2010). As mentioned earlier, if a person wishes to fulfill a commitment the obvious perquisite would be that s/he keeps it in mind. This implies that WMC should be an important factor for goal maintenance since it is a suitable mechanism to hold task relevant information (see Oberauer, 2009). Interestingly, Kane et al. (2007) found that participants with high WMC mind wandered less during demanding cognitive tasks as opposed to low-WMC participants. The authors suggest that this tendency to mind wander might lead to neglecting the task at hand. Does this apply to real life social contracts? Hofmann, Friese, and Strack (2009) suggest that high WMC allows behavior to rely on a reflective rather than an impulsive system. Very few studies have tested this in the context of enduring prolonged and cognitively challenging training programs. Conversely, Shamosh et al. (2008) found that WMC (as measured by an N-Back task) did not explain any unique variance in a delay discounting task beyond that explained by the general intelligence factor (but see Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Perrig & Meier, 2010, for N-Back having insufficient reliability as an individual differences WMC measure). To our knowledge, no study has directly assessed the role of WMC and inhibition in the ability to persevere through a highly demanding cognitive training program, yet the theoretical considerations suggest such a role. In the current study, participants underwent a battery of cognitive tasks at pre-test. We focus on four of them: two inhibition tasks (Go/No-Go, Antisaccade) and two WMC tasks (Operation Span, N-Back). Next, they committed to engage in a cognitive training program consisting of about 24 sessions. We hypothesized that better inhibition and a higher WMC will each uniquely increase the probability of completing the cognitive training programs.