دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 33817
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

گرایش اتوماتیک روش/اجتناب نسبت به مواد غذایی و دوره بیماری بی اشتهایی عصبی

عنوان انگلیسی
Automatic approach/avoidance tendencies towards food and the course of anorexia nervosa
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
33817 2015 7 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Appetite, Volume 91, 1 August 2015, Pages 28–34

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
بی اشتهایی عصبی - اتوماتیک گرایشات اجتناب رویکرد - مواد غذایی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Anorexia nervosa; Automatic approach avoidance tendencies; Food
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  گرایش اتوماتیک روش/اجتناب نسبت به مواد غذایی و  دوره بیماری بی اشتهایی عصبی

چکیده انگلیسی

Objective: The aim of the present study was to investigate the role of automatic approach/avoidance tendencies for food in Anorexia Nervosa (AN). We used a longitudinal approach and tested whether a reduction in eating disorder symptoms is associated with enhanced approach tendencies towards food and whether approach tendencies towards food at baseline are predictive for treatment outcome after one year follow up. Method: The Affective Simon Task-manikin version (AST-manikin) was administered to measure automatic approach/avoidance tendencies towards high-caloric and low-caloric food in young AN patients. Percentage underweight and eating disorder symptoms as indexed by the EDE-Q were determined both during baseline and at one year follow up. Results: At baseline anorexia patients showed an approach tendency for low caloric food, but not for high caloric food, whereas at 1 year follow up, they have an approach tendency for both high and low caloric food. Change in approach bias was neither associated with change in underweight nor with change in eating disorder symptoms. Strength of approach/avoidance tendencies was not predictive for percentage underweight. Discussion: Although approach tendencies increased after one year, approach tendencies were neither associated with concurrent change in eating disorder symptoms nor predictive for treatment success as indexed by EDE-Q. This implicates that, so far, there is no reason to add a method designed to directly target approach/avoidance tendencies to the conventional approach to treat patients with a method designed to influence the more deliberate processes in AN.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is characterized by extreme concerns of gaining weight despite existing underweight. Effectiveness of leading treatments for adolescents with AN such as Family Based Therapy (FBT) and Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) is however limited and relapse rates after recovery are high (see Byrne et al, 2011, Hay, 2013 and Lock et al, 2010). A key question is how to explain these limited success rates. One possible explanation for the relatively limited efficacy of those treatments might be their primary focus on conscious appraisals as the starting point of the interventions. Both CBT and FBT aim to target ‘explicit’ processes, by replacing dysfunctional thoughts with more effective thoughts, thus decreasing emotional distress and self-defeating behaviour. However, dual process models emphasize that next to these more explicit, deliberate processes, also more automatic, implicit, processes exist (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). Dual process models imply that behaviour is the consequence of an interplay between the reflective and the more implicit, automatic processes. In the reflective system, behaviour is guided by deliberate decision-making processes. Executive functions are needed to plan behaviour, to weigh possible consequences, and to consequently behave in an intended goal-directed manner. In the implicit (or reflexive) system, behaviour is directly activated by associative clusters in memory and this may occur spontaneously and outside of people's awareness or control. These associative clusters are formed in long-term memory through repeated experience. No executive functions or cognitive effort are needed for activating behaviour, and therefore it is assumed that this system is predictive for behaviour in situations where less cognitive resources are available (e.g., time pressure, cognitive depletion, or stress) (Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Moreover, implicit cognitions are assumed to be critically involved in habit-like, repetitive behaviours (e.g., Strack, Deutsch, 2004 and Walsh, 2013), which are typical for AN (e.g., rigid dieting). Clearly, one could argue that the refusal to eat in AN is a more deliberate process. Consequently, conventional treatment is used to address problematic behaviour in a top-down manner by taking the reflective system as the starting point. However, also bottom-up processes might play a role in AN. Several studies provided evidence that implicit, automatic processes may be involved in (un)successful dieting (Roefs et al., 2011). Accordingly, studies on attentional bias show that eating disorder patients, particularly those with BN, have an attention bias for food. While in AN evidence is mixed, in BN food stimuli might elicit greater incentive saliency, prompting the desire to eat food (Brooks, Prince, Stahl, Campbell, & Treasure, 2011). An attentional bias for food might therefore lead to increased intake (Werthmann, Jansen, & Roefs, 2014). In addition, non-successful dieters have been found to show enhanced automatic approach tendencies towards pictorial food items (Veenstra & de Jong, 2010). Although evidence is mixed and also some research points in the opposite direction (e.g. Fishbach & Shah, 2006), the view that automatic processes influence actual intake is further supported by a study that showed that implicit measures were predictive for food intake in case of low cognitive resources (Friese, Hofmann, & Wänke, 2008). Also in other areas of psychopathology a relation was found between intake and approach tendencies for the relevant substance, as for instance in alcohol (e.g. Field, Kiernan, Eastwood, & Child, 2008). Moreover, analogue studies have shown that experimentally reducing automatic chocolate-approach tendencies also reduced participants' craving for chocolates (Kemps, Tiggemann, Martin, & Elliott, 2013). Whereas in disinhibited eating and addiction, heightened automatic approach tendencies may be involved, in AN, the opposite might be the crucial problem. The common approach tendencies for food might be absent in individuals with AN. In this way, AN patients are more similar to individuals with anxiety disorders, in that they too show an avoidance tendency away from disorder-relevant (threatening) stimuli (e.g., Rinck & Becker, 2007). Avoidance of high caloric food can become a well-established habit and very resistant to change (Walsh, 2013). The successful restriction of food intake in AN patients, even under conditions that typically impair self-control might then thus be explained by assuming that automatic responses towards food are weakened or perhaps absent among AN patients. In line with such a view, recent research using an indirect approach avoidance task, provided evidence indicating that indeed the common approach bias towards high caloric food was attenuated in AN patients compared to non-symptomatic controls (Veenstra & de Jong, 2011). Possibly conventional treatment has a limited effect on this type of more automatic processes. It could therefore be hypothesized that treatment success is limited if these relevant automatic processes remain unaffected. In other words, limited treatment success in AN patients might be associated with a failure to enhance automatic approach tendencies towards food items. As a first step to examine whether the efficacy of the treatment of AN indeed critically depends on its success in normalizing the approach tendencies towards food, the present study tested whether approach/avoidance tendencies change over time, and examined whether the reduction in AN symptoms was associated with an increase in approach tendencies towards food. Moreover, if a lack of approach tendencies towards food indeed plays an important role in the persistence of eating disorder symptoms, relatively weak approach tendencies at baseline might be an important moderator of treatment success. Therefore, the next aim of the study was to test whether (low) approach tendencies towards food at baseline predicted (worse) treatment outcome at one-year follow up. The current study used a longitudinal design, in which approach tendencies for food and eating pathology of a large group of AN patients were measured at the moment of intake and at a fixed subsequent assessment at one-year follow up. In short, the major aim of the current study was to test whether (i) approach tendencies change between moment of intake and one year follow-up, (ii) a reduction in eating disorder symptoms is associated with enhanced approach tendencies towards food, and (iii) approach tendencies towards food at baseline are predictive for treatment outcome after one year follow up.