دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 29843
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

یک مطالعه کیفی بررسی درک نوجوانان از خشونت، زورگویی و خشونت

عنوان انگلیسی
A qualitative study investigating adolescents' understanding of aggression, bullying and violence
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
29843 2013 9 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Children and Youth Services Review, Volume 35, Issue 4, April 2013, Pages 685–693

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
- قلدری - پرخاشگری - خشونت - گروه نوجوانی - تمرکز -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Bullying,Aggression,Violence,Adolescence,Focus groups,
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  یک مطالعه کیفی بررسی درک نوجوانان از خشونت، زورگویی و خشونت

چکیده انگلیسی

There is a general consistency across the research literature regarding the definition of bullying. This has filtered down into the construction of governmental and school anti-bullying policies around the world. However, research suggests that children and adolescents are failing to accurately identify cases of bullying. This in turn has implications upon the accuracy of our perception of the extent of the problem of bullying within schools. The current study aimed to investigate how 11–17 year olds understand and differentiate between terms relating to interpersonal peer aggression, violence and bullying. Fifty-seven (twenty male, thirty-seven female) participants were recruited via an opportunity sample. Participants took part in focus group interviews within which they were asked to provide a definition for a list of words relating to both traditional and cyber forms of aggression, bullying and violence. Thematic analysis revealed that the participants held a shared understanding of the terms relating to aggression, bullying and violence. Participants defined each term by describing the behaviors involved, their perception of the level of control the perpetrators of each type of negative peer interaction have and the perception of those involved. The implications of these findings for both policy and future research are discussed.

مقدمه انگلیسی

There is a general academic consensus regarding the definition of bullying; a specific subtype of aggressive behavior defined as, ‘intentional negative behavior that… occurs with some repetitiveness and is directed against a person who has difficulty defending himself or herself’ (Olweus, 2011. p.151). Bullying is differentiated into two broad behavioral subcategories of direct and indirect bullying ( Carbone-Lopez et al., 2010, Hampel et al., 2009 and Marini et al., 2006). Direct bullying refers to the use of overt behaviors which include physical attacks such as hitting, pushing, kicking (physical bullying; Wang, Iannotti, Luk, & Nansel, 2010), name-calling and threatening behavior (verbal bullying; Eliot et al., 2010 and Wang et al., 2010). In contrast, indirect bullying includes the manipulation of social relationships in order to socially isolate the victim ( Carbone-Lopez et al., 2010). This type of psychological bullying, also known as relational bullying involves behaviors such as rumor spreading, sexual gestures and verbal abuse and is carried out within the victim's social group ( Corvo & deLara, 2010). Relational bullying can also be perpetrated via a technological device (termed cyber bullying: Marsh et al., 2010, Perren et al., 2010, Subrahmanyam and Greenfield, 2008 and Slonje and Smith, 2008). Cyber bullying, although previously isolated within the literature and investigated independently is rapidly becoming an increasingly dominant form of bullying ( Calvete et al., 2010, Wade and Beran, 2011 and Wang et al., 2012). The behavioral and affective overlap between traditional (behaviors which are not perpetrated via technology) and cyber forms of bullying are now being investigated ( Hay et al., 2010, Vandebosch and Van Cleemput, 2008 and Wade and Beran, 2011). Victims of both cyber and traditional forms of childhood bullying have been found to suffer from a range of negative physical and psychological consequences including decreased self-esteem (Dukes, Stein, & Zane, 2009), involvement in delinquent behavior (Lösel & Bender, 2011), increased drug use (Carbone-Lopez et al., 2010), perceived lack of personal safety at school (Esbensen & Carson, 2009) and lower educational attainment (Beran et al., 2008 and Rothon et al., 2011). The effects of bullying can also impact upon long-term well being (Ttofi & Farrington, 2008), with victims facing an increased risk of suffering from depression (Roth, Coles, & Heimberg, 2002), anxiety (Lösel & Bender, 2011) and relational difficulties in adulthood (Carlisle & Rofes, 2007). International research consistently signifies that childhood and adolescent bullying (following the definition outlined by Olweus, 2011) is a global problem (Mlisa et al., 2008, Thornberg, 2010 and Wang et al., 2012). In acknowledgment of the severity of the impact of bullying, the past decade has observed a notable increase in the construction of anti-bullying legislation around the world. Arguably, the most notable increase is within the United States of America. Although there is no federal legal requirement to do so, since 1999, forty-six of the fifty-one states now have legislation requiring schools to construct an anti-bullying policy (eight of which have been constructed since 2010). Within England and Wales governmental policy dictates that each school must construct and implement an anti-bullying policy (The Education and Inspections Act, 2006 [89]: 1.b, 3, 5). Reflecting the definition of bullying reported within the research literature, the Department for Education (DfE) in England and Wales defines bullying as a, ‘behaviour by an individual or group, repeated over time, that intentionally hurts another individual or group either physically or emotionally’; with the definition of cyber bullying being extended to include the use of technology and victimization occurring, ‘24/7, with a potentially bigger audience’ (DfE, 2011: 4). This definition is used to inform the construction of anti-bullying policies within schools and so effects how educators inform pupils about what bullying involves. This definition is also reflected within the media and within online resources designed to inform children and adolescents about bullying (BBC Newsround, 2011 and BeatBullying, 2012). Despite the consistency in the definition of bullying across the research literature, governmental and individual school policies and the media, research suggests that children and adolescents may hold a less consistent understanding of the phenomenon to those researching the problem (Frisén et al., 2008, Maunder et al., 2010, Monks and Smith, 2006, Rodkin and Berger, 2008, Smith et al., 2002 and Vaillancourt et al., 2008). One method researchers have utilized to investigate adolescents' understanding of bullying is to compare reported involvement rates between samples provided with a definition of bullying compared with those that are not provided with a definition of the term. The findings from this area of research highlight inconsistencies between the understanding held by academics compared to children and adolescents. Vaillancourt et al. (2008) divided a sample of 8–18 year old participants into two groups; one of which received a definition of the term ‘bully’ (following the criteria defined by Olweus, 2011) and the other was asked to indicate their involvement in bullying based upon their own understanding of the term. They found that the group who were provided with the definition reported lower prevalence rates of victimization compared to those who were not provided with one; suggesting that this sample of participants considered bullying to include a different set of criteria than the widely held definition. Indeed, when the group who were not provided with a definition was asked to define bullying, they did not consistently report the criteria of repeated behaviors, intentionality, nor a power imbalance between aggressor and victim. In another study Frisén et al. (2008) simply asked 13 year old participants to define their understanding of what constitutes a bullying relationship. Like Vaillancourt et al.'s (2008) sample, the definitions offered by the 13 year olds were inconsistent and did not reflect the definition adopted by academics and policy makers; 70% of the sample did not mention repeated behaviors and 81% did not mention a power imbalance. Across the sample, the participants were more likely to mention the behaviors involved (indirect, physical and verbal) rather than the criteria by which they define these behaviors as bullying. By not perceiving the criteria of intentionality and repetition as integral to the definition, children and adolescents may perceive one-off acts of violence and aggression as bullying. Thus, when investigating the prevalence of bullying, without providing participants with a definition, participants may base their indication of victimization upon criteria which may differ from the specific criteria of interest to the researchers. However, the findings from Frisén et al. (2008) and Vaillancourt et al.'s (2008) samples may not be representative of children and adolescents understanding in other participant samples. Children and adolescents' understanding of the term, ‘bullying’ may be subject to cultural interpretations or translations of the term. For example, the term ‘bullying’ itself does not translate across all languages to incorporate aspects of physical, verbal and relational bullying; with translations within the Spanish language not including the latter form of bullying for example. Similarly, the term ‘cyber bullying’ (which in English refers to bullying which occurs via any technological device) when directly translated into different languages does not consistently hold the same meaning as it does within the English language. For example, its meaning in Italian, German and Spanish is restricted to incorporate bullying behaviors using the internet only (Nocentini et al., 2010 and Vandebosch and Van Cleemput, 2008). Influences on children's understanding of bullying also include the child's experiences as they mature and witness the bullying behaviors of others (Monks and Smith, 2006 and Monks et al., 2003) and the education they receive within school; which may make them aware of subtypes of bullying which they have not directly witnessed themselves. Thus, when comparing findings of international research, caution should be taken before generalizing the findings of research which does not provide participants with a definition of the term under investigation. Further variations between the understanding held by academics and children and adolescences are that of the behaviors utilized and the individuals who are involved in bullying relationships. Rodkin and Berger (2008) suggest that children may hold a specific schema of those who are involved in bullying. They asked a sample of 10–11 year olds to nominate peers who bully other children. They found that participants tended to nominate boys who were physically and relationally aggressive as being bullies; suggesting that children may hold a specific schema of the gender of a bully. Although this finding reflects research which suggests that the prevalence of male involvement in bullying is higher than female involvement (Carbone-Lopez et al., 2010), the findings of other studies suggest that females are just as likely as males to engage in bullying; but are more likely to use relational compared to physical strategies (Smith et al., 2008). The findings of Rodkin and Berger's (2008) study may therefore be a reflection of what children are witnessing and experiencing at school; reflecting only the overt behaviors displayed by males. Therefore, caution should be taken when generalizing these findings to participants from other cultures and even different participant samples as their experiences and what behaviors they witness may vary. A further inconsistency between the understanding held by academics and children and adolescents is that regarding the behaviors they define as being utilized within bullying relationships. Maunder et al. (2010) found that when their sample of 12–16 year old participants was asked to identify behaviors from a list, indirect behaviors were less likely than direct behaviors to be identified as bullying. This perception does not reflect the research evidence which suggests that indirect forms of bullying are more prevalent than direct physical forms (Carbone-Lopez et al., 2010). This area of research highlights discrepancies between adolescents' perception of bullying and the research evidence collected regarding those involved in bullying and the behaviors they experience. This discrepancy however may reflect the ambiguity of interpreting the purpose of behaviors associated with relational and indirect bullying. Characterized by behaviors such as teasing and rumor spreading, those not involved in indirect bullying may perceive such behaviors as playful and not intended to harm the victim; therefore they may be less likely to indicate them as bullying behaviors within questionnaires. However, the recipients of these behaviors, regardless of the intent (which may be unknown to the victim), perceive them negatively and report them as bullying. Therefore, the perspectives of those involved and those commenting on their perception of bullying may present differing representations of the perceived severity and intent of the ambiguous behaviors associated with indirect forms of bullying. These inconsistencies between academics' and children and adolescents' understanding of bullying suggest that there are problems within the anti-bullying education process, whereby pupils do not fully understand what behaviors do and do not constitute bullying. The issue which arises from these differing understandings is that if an inaccurate picture of the prevalence of the problem is being reported within schools, this in turn will impact on the perceived severity of the problem. Subsequently, as Maunder et al. (2010) suggest, this will lead to insufficient attention and resources being allocated to schools to tackle bullying. At present, it is unclear where and how children and adolescents acquire and develop their understanding of bullying and how they differentiate this repeated use of aggression with general aggressive and violent behaviors. Despite the increased research attention focusing on bullying and its effects on those involved, to date there are few studies which have investigated how groups of young people specifically within England construct their understanding of the term bullying (Maunder et al., 2010 and Thornberg, 2010). Furthermore no qualitative research has been conducted to investigate how children and adolescents differentiate their understanding of bullying from their understanding of general interpersonal violence and aggression; two terms which are used interchangeably across the research literature to refer to harmful physical and verbal acts and which do not conform to a consistent definition. This study therefore adopted a qualitative approach to investigate how children and adolescents construct and differentiate between the terms aggression, bullying and violence (which will cumulatively be referred to as negative peer behaviors or interactions). Adopting a qualitative, rather than quantitative method allows participants to express their understanding of negative peer interactions based upon their own perception and experiences rather than being restricted or influenced by the preconceived ideas of the researchers investigating the topic. To investigate this issue the following questions were addressed: 1. How do adolescents between the ages of 11–17 years define concepts relating to traditional and cyber forms bullying, violence and aggression? 2. How do adolescents differentiate between bullying and general interpersonal violence and aggression?

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

The 11–18 year olds within the current sample were able to identify the characteristics of bullying relationships which are adopted across the academic literature and by the DfE. Further, the terms bullying, aggression and violence were defined and differentiated by the repetition of behaviors and the perception of control the perpetrators have over their actions. However, themes emerged from the talk which may inhibit efforts to encourage the reduction of negative peer interactions. These related to the perception that the victims of bullying are different from the majority and that certain individuals cannot control their behavior when experiencing anger; thus mitigating the responsibility for their behaviors. The findings of the current study suggest that future research in needed to investigate, 1) the perception that children and adolescent's hold of victims within bullying relationships and the effect this has on victim, 2) how children and adolescents establish the intentionality of others' behaviors, 3) how this perception of intentionality effects their response to negative behaviors. It is important that research focuses on how these understandings are being developed in order to address how to effectively inform young people about aggression, bullying and violence.