تبعیض بین بی اشتهایی و بولیمیا با هوس های غذایی: پیامدها برای "موفقیت" در مقابل "شکست" در محدود کردن رژیم غذایی
کد مقاله | سال انتشار | تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی |
---|---|---|
32526 | 2009 | 7 صفحه PDF |
Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)
Journal : Appetite, Volume 52, Issue 3, June 2009, Pages 588–594
چکیده انگلیسی
Food cravings are subjective, motivational states thought to induce binge eating among eating disorder patients. This study compared food cravings across eating disorders. Women (N = 135) diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, restrictive (ANR) or binge–purging (ANBP) types, or bulimia nervosa, non-purging (BNNP) or purging (BNP) types completed measures of food cravings. Discriminant analysis yielded two statistically significant functions. The first function differentiated between all the four group pairs except ANBP and BNNP, with levels of various food-craving dimensions successively increasing for ANR, ANBP, BNNP, and BNP participants. The second function differentiated between ANBP and BNNP participants. Overall, the functions improved classification accuracy above chance level (44% fewer errors). The findings suggest that cravings are more strongly associated with loss of control over eating than with dietary restraint tendencies.
مقدمه انگلیسی
Food cravings are prevalent in the general population and in patients with eating disorders. Generally defined as an all-consuming desire or strong yearning to eat, food cravings are of particular interest to researchers and clinicians because they are thought to induce binge eating. Formally proposed in the late 1970s, the dietary restraint model (e.g., Cooper, 1997; Fairburn & Cooper, 1993; Heatherton & Polivy, 1992; Vitousek, 1996) purports that individuals who chronically attempt to maintain strict dietary control are at high risk for becoming temporarily disinhibited in maintaining their control, which often results in a binge eating episode ( Polivy & Herman, 1985; Ruderman, 1986; Schotte, Cools, & McNally, 1990). In other words, this model describes a self-fueling cycle in which temporary caloric restriction and homeostatic imbalances, which are common in individuals struggling with eating disorders, causes cravings to eat. Food cravings, in turn, can drive individuals to binge eat ( Booth, Lewis, & Blair, 1990; Cepeda-Benito & Gleaves, 2001; Fairburn & Cooper, 1982). Since its initial conceptualization, researchers have elaborated upon the dietary restraint model by examining the influence of mediating and moderating factors that predict binge eating, predominately in non-eating disordered samples (Fairburn, 1997 and Wilson, 1999). The factors that often lead to binge eating include breaches of dietary restraint and eating “forbidden” foods (Abraham & Beaumont, 1982), free time and being alone (Johnson, 1985; Pyle, Mitchell, & Eckert, 1981), viewing diet-related commercials and advertisements (Strauss, Kriepe, & Doyle, 1994; Warren, Strauss, Taska, & Sullivan, 2005) and strong emotional states (e.g., Cools, Schotte, & McNally, 1992; Cooper & Bowskill, 1986; Fairburn, 1997, Schotte et al., 1990 and Wilson, 1999). Research that investigates the relationships between food cravings and binge eating is necessary to understand the role of food cravings in the mechanisms that control appetite and eating in women with eating disorders. The scarce research done to this date with clinical samples has nonetheless yielded important findings. Bulik, Sullivan, Carter, McIntosh, and Joyce (1998) found that cue-exposure/response prevention interventions may reduce craving reactivity in bulimic patients. A few pilot investigations have shown that reductions in cue reactivity are associated with declines in binge eating (Jansen, 2001). In comparison to asymptomatic individuals, women with a history of anorexia nervosa who binged and purged were more likely to report “uncontrollable desires to eat certain foods or types of food” and “strong urges to eat a specific food” (Gendall, Sullivan, Joyce, & Bulik, 1997). Waters, Hill, and Waller (2001) found that experiencing “a strong urge or desire for a particular food” was followed by bingeing about 50% of the time. Similarly, Engelberg, Gauvin, and Steiger (2005) reported that whereas dietary restraint did not necessarily precede binge eating, elevated restraint preceded “strong” cravings and the probability of bingeing. Despite a body of research theorizing and supporting the role of food cravings in precipitating binge eating, other findings make drawing definite conclusions difficult. Several studies have downplayed the impact of dietary restraint in bingeing and purging (Cooper, Clark, & Fairburn, 1993; Heatherton & Polivy, 1992; Lowe et al., 1996; Lowe, Gleaves, & Murphy-Eberenz, 1998). Lowe et al. (1996) found that dieting intensity was not related to the severity of binge eating among individuals with bulimia. Moreover, although individuals with eating disorder symptoms report more intense and frequent cravings than asymptomatic individuals (Jarosz, Dobal, Wilson, & Schram, 2007), a very small proportion of food cravers may actually engage in binge eating (Gendall, Sullivan et al., 1997). Whereas Nederkoorn, Smulders, and Jansen (2000) found that cue-induced craving was correlated with food intake in a non-clinical sample, reductions in craving reactivity to food cues do not necessarily reduce binge eating in individuals with bulimia (Bulik et al., 1998). Cepeda-Benito and Gleaves (2001) hypothesized that different methodologies across studies may explain why the data are conflicted with regard to the nature of the association between food cravings, dietary restraint, and binge eating. These authors noted that associations between dietary restraint and food craving were negative in samples of successful dieters and non-existing in samples made up of successful and unsuccessful dieters (e.g., Cohen, Sherwin, & Fleming, 1987; Harvey, Wing, & Mullen, 1993; Hill, Weaver, & Blundell, 1991). Conversely, studies that used unsuccessful dieters, or restrained eaters with high tendencies towards disinhibition of restraint, reported greater cravings and food intake in reaction to food cues and fasting than unrestrained eaters (e.g., Fedoroff, Polivy, & Herman, 1997; Tuomisto et al., 1999). That is, in the studies reviewed, positive associations between dietary restraint and food craving may have been defined to a larger extent by whether or not individuals breached their diets than by the intensity of their diets. Similarly, Stice, Cooper, Schoeller, Tappe, and Lowe (2007) noted recently that scores on dietary restraint scales do not correlate with objective indicators of “moderate- to long-term dietary restriction,” and that increases in dietary restriction over moderate (6 weeks) to long (up to 3 years) periods have been found to decrease binge eating in controlled studies (e.g., Burton & Stice, 2006; Stice, Shaw, Burton, & Wade, 2006). That is, the relationship between dietary restrain and loss of control over eating appears to be moderated by whether or not the individual is a successful dieter (i.e., able to maintain moderate- to long-term caloric restriction). The present study tested the hypothesis that food cravings are highly prevalent in individuals known to have high tendencies to temporarily restrain their diets but lose control and binge eat, whereas individuals who are successful dieters and restrict their caloric intake over long periods are unlikely to experience cravings. The overarching goal of this study was to examine whether food cravings can differentiate women who desire to restrict their caloric intake but who differ with regard to their tendency to engage in bingeing. Using discriminatory factor analysis (DFA), our goal was to determine whether scale scores from the Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait (Cepeda-Benito, Gleaves, Williams, & Erath, 2000) could differentiate between women diagnosed with anorexia nervosa (AN), both restrictive (ANR) and bingeing/purging type (ANBP), and bulimia nervosa, purging (BNP) and non-purging type (BNNP). It was predicted that individuals diagnosed with BNP and BNNP (who by definition binge eat) would report more intense food cravings than individuals who by definition do not binge eat (ANR). Given that some individuals diagnosed with ANBP may engage in bingeing and/or purging, but not necessarily both, we anticipated that ANBP individuals would report greater levels of food craving (and binge eating) than ANR participants, but lower levels of craving (and binge eating) than BN patients. A directional hypothesis regarding how cravings would differ between BNP and BNNP based on likelihood of bingeing was not readily apparent. However, to the extent that the presence of purging behavior may be an indicator of greater psychopathology and bingeing intensity (Gleaves, Williamson, & Barker, 1993; Tobin, Griffing, & Griffing, 1997), we ventured to predict that BNP would report greater cravings than BNNP participants.