دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 33414
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

خشم اتفاقی و تمایل به ارزیابی

عنوان انگلیسی
Incidental anger and the desire to evaluate
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
33414 2011 11 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Volume 116, Issue 1, September 2011, Pages 55–65

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
احساسات - خشم - تصمیم گیری - ارزیابی - عاطفه - قضاوت - خلق و خوی - انتخاب وظیفه - اولویت -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Emotion; Anger; Decision-making; Evaluation; Affect; Judgment; Mood; Task choice; Preference
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  خشم اتفاقی و تمایل به ارزیابی

چکیده انگلیسی

Our results indicate that people experiencing incidental anger are more likely than people in neutral and other emotional states to prefer to perform evaluative tasks, even though their anger may bias the evaluations they make. Induced anger increased participants’ desire to evaluate others’ ideas (Experiment 1) and made the evaluations of those ideas more negative in valence (Experiment 2). Anger increased the appeal of evaluating ideas when evaluations were expected to be largely negative but not when evaluations were expected to be positive (Experiments 3 and 4). Mediation analyses revealed that this willingness to evaluate when angry stems from a belief that evaluating others can leave angry people in a positive mood. Because people are often free to decide when to perform the tasks required of them, this tendency may have implications for how and when ideas are evaluated.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Many jobs include the task of evaluating others’ ideas. Since people often have the freedom to decide when to perform each of the many tasks required of them, they could choose to evaluate ideas when they are most capable of doing so objectively and thoroughly. For example, an executive who is angry about receiving a speeding ticket could decide to wait to review proposals for new business plans until his anger has subsided and he could approach the task more objectively. But would he wait for his anger to subside, or would he instead evaluate the plans while angry? Emotions can be differentiated from one another by their “cognitive appraisals” (Smith and Ellsworth, 1985, Smith and Ellsworth, 1988 and Roseman, 1991). For example, whereas some emotions, like anger, co-occur with a sense of certainty and blaming others, other emotions are associated with a sense of uncertainty (e.g. fear) or blaming the self (e.g. guilt). The appraisals associated with an emotion can sometimes seep from an initial emotional state into a target of judgment that has nothing to do with the initial emotion (Keltner et al., 1993, Lerner and Keltner, 2000 and Lerner and Keltner, 2001; Lerner & Tiedens, 2006). In one of the earliest demonstrations of this phenomenon, Keltner et al. (1993) showed that when people were induced to feel angry, an emotion associated with blame, they were more blaming of another person who made a social faux pas, than when they were sad, an emotion associated with the sense that no one is to blame. Similarly, Lerner and Keltner (2001) found that emotions associated with a lack of control (such as fear) resulted in higher risk assessments of unrelated events than did emotions associated with a greater sense of control and certainty (e.g. anger). Thus, the seeping of appraisals associated with emotions into unrelated judgments is one way in which emotions dramatically affect how people perceive and evaluate the world around them. Emotions and their associated appraisal can also influence how deeply people process information, such that people generally think more systematically when experiencing sadness and rely more on heuristics when experiencing happiness or anger (Bodenhausen et al., 1994 and Tiedens and Linton, 2001, but see Moons & Mackie, 2007; for an exception). Moreover, emotions can affect how willing people are to listen to and heed advice (Gino & Schweitzer, 2008) and how much they trust other people, even people not responsible for their anger (Dunn & Schweitzer, 2005). As such, emotions color how positively and thoroughly people evaluate stimuli, even if the stimuli are unrelated to the source of the underlying emotion (e.g., Bower, 1991 and Lerner and Tiedens, 2006). Although prior work has documented the effects of discrete emotional states on how people evaluate stimuli, it has not examined the impact of those emotional states on the decision to evaluate those stimuli. This paper investigates whether people are most likely to evaluate others’ ideas when in neutral emotional states or if they systematically choose to evaluate ideas under emotional conditions that may not always result in the most thorough or objective of evaluations. We propose that people experiencing anger, even incidental anger triggered by an experience unrelated to the current environment (Bodenhausen et al., 1994), are more likely than those in neutral and even other negative emotional states to choose to evaluate others’ ideas. We focus on the emotion of anger for two reasons. First, anger has an oppositional component (Lerner and Tiedens, 2006 and Roseman et al., 1994). It is associated with an action tendency associated with “moving against” other people or obstacles (Frijda, Kuipers, & ter Schure, 1989) and is often displaced, such that people often act aggressively toward someone other than the person who initially instigated anger (Fenigstein and Buss, 1974 and Holmes, 1972). To the extent that evaluating others’ ideas constitutes an oppositional behavior, anger should influence the propensity to evaluate others’ ideas. The second reason that we focus on anger is that it is likely to have particularly strong effects on judgments of others’ ideas. People in angry moods evaluate stimuli more negatively than do people in neutral emotion conditions (Bower, 1991) and they process information more heuristically than do people experiencing neutral emotions or people experiencing sadness (Bower, 1991, Lerner et al., 1998 and Tiedens and Linton, 2001). We therefore focus on anger because prior research suggests both that it should affect people’s tendency to evaluate others and that evaluations made while experiencing anger should differ systematically from evaluations made in other emotional states. We focus our analysis on incidental emotions because doing so makes for a conservative test of the relationship between anger and the desire to evaluate others’ ideas. As Gino and Schweitzer (2008) point out, integral emotions are likely to have stronger effects on decisions and are less likely to decay over time than are incidental emotions. Integral emotions may also lead people to seek out opportunities very consciously to evaluate another person’s ideas if that person caused them to experience anger. The influence of incidental emotions is therefore more subtle and more surprising. In testing our hypotheses, we contribute to the broader literature on emotion by exploring whether incidental emotion may lead people to be more likely to perform some types of activities (e.g., evaluating others’ ideas) in which their performance is likely to be affected by incidental emotions. We also contribute to the decision-making literature by proposing that emotions can not only directly affect how people make decisions but also that emotions influence when people engage in a particular kind of decision making or evaluation. Anger and the willingness to strike out against others Considerable research has shown that anger is often a precursor to aggression (e.g., Berkowitz, 1989, Berkowitz, 1990 and Berkowitz, 2000), which Berkowitz (1993) defines as “behavior, either physical or symbolic, that is carried out with the intention to harm someone” (p. 11). Most often, this aggression is directed at the source of the anger. However, anger can also lead people to move against people or obstacles, even if the people or obstacles are not responsible for their anger (Fenigstein and Buss, 1974, Frijda et al., 1989 and Holmes, 1972). People induced to feel anger have been shown to be more punitive toward those violating social conventions (Goldberg et al., 1999 and Lerner et al., 1998), more blaming of others in situations unrelated to the source of their anger (Keltner et al., 1993), and more seeking of opportunities to argue and fight with others (Young, Tiedens, Jung, & Tsai, 2011). To the extent that evaluating others’ ideas entails striking out at others by providing negative feedback, people in angry moods should find more appeal in evaluating others’ ideas than should other people. They may see judging others negatively as an outlet for their anger and, therefore, a way to alleviate their angry feelings (Bushman, Baumeister, & Phillips, 2001). Anger may therefore remove people’s inhibitions about evaluating others because it reduces people’s aversion to making negative judgments.