دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 31543
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

سازماندهی کردن و منافع خاص: مشخص کردن پیوستگی از عصبی معمولی به اختلال طیف اوتیسم

عنوان انگلیسی
Systemizing and special interests: Characterizing the continuum from neurotypical to autism spectrum disorder
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
31543 2014 9 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Learning and Individual Differences, Volume 29, January 2014, Pages 98–105

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
اختلال طیف اوتیسم - سیندروم اسپرگر - منافع خاص - بزرگسالان -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Autism spectrum disorder; Asperger syndrome; Special interests; Adults; Systemizing.
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  سازماندهی کردن و منافع خاص: مشخص کردن پیوستگی از عصبی معمولی به اختلال طیف اوتیسم

چکیده انگلیسی

Special interests have been studied in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) but not in adults. Using an online survey, it was found that individuals with ASD reported more intense interests in systemizable domains, relative to neurotypical adults. Self-reported systemizing preference was correlated with intensity of interest in systemizable domains both for those with ASD and for neurotypical young adults. Few gender differences were found in the neurotypical group in the expected categories of machines, technology and vehicles, where gender differences have been found in children. Gender differences in these categories did appear for the ASD group. We propose a strength-based model of special interests, with the hobbies of neurotypical forming a continuum with the special interests of ASD.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Among individuals with Asperger syndrome, repetitive behaviors often occur in the form of intense interests which may be considered unusual or atypical in their content (e.g., World War II airplanes; see Asperger, 1991, Attwood, 2003 and Winter-Messiers, 2007). These are sometimes called restricted or circumscribed interests (e.g., Klin, Danovitch, Merz, & Volkmar, 2007). Following Winter-Messiers (2007), we use the term “special interests” because it promotes a strength-based approach and it is also the term most frequently used by individuals on the Internet discussion forums from which we recruited survey participants in the current study. We describe and defend the view that special interests are not primarily repetitive behaviors, but reflect information processing styles and cognitive strengths (Baron-Cohen, 2002). The interests and hobbies of neurotypical individuals (i.e., those without ASD, Attwood, 1998 and Winter-Messiers, 2007) frequently reflect individuals' cognitive-personality styles, and we thus propose that the special interests of those with Asperger syndrome or on the autism spectrum lie on a continuum with neurotypical hobbies. This leads to two primary hypotheses: (1) that the content of special interests exists on a continuum between neurotypical individuals and individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and (2) that special interests reflect information processing styles, such that the interests of individuals with ASD are correlated with systemizing ability, and interests of neurotypicals with mentalizing ability, as measured by the Systemizing Quotient (Wheelwright et al., 2006) and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001). Past research has focused primarily on the special interests of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), who develop these interests as early as 1–4 years of age (Attwood, 2003, Bashe and Kirby, 2001 and Moore and Goodson, 2003), although some interests cannot be reliably scored on certain assessments until age 4 (Moore & Goodson, 2003). Approximately 75–90% of individuals with mild to moderate autism or Asperger syndrome develop one or more special interests (Bashe and Kirby, 2001 and Klin et al., 2007). Other individuals on the autism spectrum, including those with PDD-NOS (Sturm, Fernell, & Gillberg, 2004) and Rett syndrome (Mazzocco et al., 1998), also exhibit intense interests. Special interests are often manifested in efforts to collect objects or information relevant to the interest area, which can require extensive amounts of time (Bashe & Kirby, 2001). Parents often find special interests the most difficult to accommodate of autistic behaviors due to their intensity (Mercier, Mottron, & Belleville, 2000). Unlike other behaviors, special interests do not lessen with age (Fecteau, Mottron, Berthiaume, & Burack, 2003) and the number of interests may increase as the individual approaches adulthood (Bashe & Kirby, 2001). Among children with ASD, special interests often reflect exceptional abilities, such as systemizing and heightened attention to detail. Systemizing is the drive to explore, analyze, and construct systems (Baron-Cohen, Richler, Bisarya, Gurunathan, & Wheelwright, 2003). Domains that are amenable to systemizing are rule-based and predictable, facilitating detection of input-operation–output relationships (Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Ashwin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009). The special interests of children with ASD frequently occur in systemizable domains, such as in mechanical, collectible, natural, and numerical systems (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999). Neurotypical (NT) individuals also develop special interests. While up to 90% of individuals with ASD develop special interests (Bashe and Kirby, 2001 and Klin et al., 2007), about 30% of NT children develop an “extremely intense interest” (DeLoache, Simcock, & Macari, 2007). Special interests in NT children may appear as early as 1–2 years of age. Like children with ASD, the special interests of NT children reflect their cognitive strengths. NT children are often preoccupied by interests in people, imaginative play, and the social environment more broadly (Attwood, 2003 and DeLoache et al., 2007). These interests are consistent with strong mentalizing ability (i.e., the capacity to understand and attribute mental states to others, Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a and Frith et al., 1991). While individuals with ASD often have higher systemizing ability than NT individuals (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003), NT individuals generally have higher mentalizing abilities (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a and Baron-Cohen et al., 2001b). Furthermore, NT females tend to have higher mentalizing ability than NT males (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a and Baron-Cohen et al., 2001b), while NT males generally have higher systemizing ability than NT females (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003). Little is currently known about special interests in adulthood. Understanding adults' special interests is important for illuminating the positive role that interests play in adult life, such as rewarding careers (Attwood, 2003, Grandin and Duffy, 2008 and Jackson, 2002). No studies yet compare the interests of individuals with ASD to those of NT individuals. The current study examines the content of special interests held by adults with ASD and NT adults, with the expectation that individuals with ASD will hold more intense interests in domains previously identified as systemizable, such as machines and technology, sorting, categorizing, and organizing, factual and numerical information, collecting, and the sciences. We also expect that NT individuals will hold more intense interests in mentalizing domains, such as in people and sports and games (as discussion of sports fosters social affiliation, see Mueller, Agamanolis, & Picard, 2003). The current study will also assess participants' systemizing and mentalizing abilities, with the goal of identifying whether interest categories previously identified as systemizable correlate with high systemizing ability, and likewise whether interest categories expected to rely on mentalizing correlate with high mentalizing ability. By identifying cognitive abilities that contribute to special interests, we hope to develop a strength-based continuum model of special interests, where the interests of individuals with ASD and NT individuals coexist and vary depending on systemizing and mentalizing ability (see Fig. 3 and Discussion section). A strength-based model of special interests can inform the development of educational and therapeutic programs that capitalize on individual abilities and use special interests as a medium for learning other important skills. We also hope to better understand gender differences in special interests. Winter-Messiers (2007) reported that ASD girls were more likely than boys to have more neurotypical interests, such as a 10 year-old girl being interested in horses. Attwood (2003) also noted ASD girls' intense interests in dolls, animals and fiction, but remarked on some aberrant qualities of these interests. Girls didn't use their dolls to play with others, but would self-play with a large doll collection. An interest in animals could become consuming; girls would want to act like animals, or to sleep in a stable. Interests in fiction included collecting and wanting to contact the author. The focus of the writing interest was not related to school success or an anticipated career. ASD is well known to be less frequent in girls, which has been hypothesized to reflect protective aspects of girls' socialization (e.g., Constantino & Todd, 2003) or lower fetal testosterone (Auyeung et al., 2006). It would be useful to know if systemizing and/or a person's autism traits predicted their type of interests independently of gender. Gender differences in interests also exist in typically developing children. In the study of DeLoache et al. (2007), half of the intense interests that parents reported for boys were for vehicles, trains, and machines, and another 27% were for balls, dinosaurs, and tools. Girls' intense interests were clothes/dressing up, babies, and tea sets. These gender differences in interests were observed in the youngest children studied (approximately 1 year old). Since knowledge of gender stereotypes, implicit or explicit, does not occur until 18 months or later, DeLoache et al. speculated that biological factors, such as fetal testosterone, play a role. Gender differences in special interests have not previously been studied in adult persons with ASD. Our secondary hypothesis is that we will see continuity between childhood and adulthood. We thus expect that both ASD and neurotypical males will have interests in more systemizable domains than will ASD and neurotypical females. Understanding gender differences in special interests during adulthood is important, as it can provide a means to adapt skill development in the workplace and higher educational settings to individual cognitive styles.