دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 31246
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

آلکسیتیمیا در اسکیزوفرنی: ارتباط با شناخت اعصاب و ناراحتی عاطفی

عنوان انگلیسی
Alexithymia in schizophrenia: Associations with neurocognition and emotional distress
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
31246 2014 6 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Psychiatry Research, Volume 218, Issues 1–2, 15 August 2014, Pages 1–6

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
اسکیزوفرنی - آلکسی تایمیا - افسردگی - اضطراب - شناخت اجتماعی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Schizophrenia, Alexithymia, Neurocognition, Depression, Anxiety, Social cognition, MATRICS,
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  آلکسیتیمیا در اسکیزوفرنی: ارتباط با شناخت اعصاب و ناراحتی عاطفی

چکیده انگلیسی

While alexithymia, or difficulties identifying and describing affect, has been commonly observed in schizophrenia, little is known about its causes and correlates. To test the hypothesis that deficits in emotion identification and expression result from, or are at least related to, deficits in neurocognition and affective symptoms, we assessed alexithymia using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), symptoms using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), and neurocognition using the MATRICS battery among 65 adults with schizophrenia spectrum disorders in a non-acute phase of illness. Partial correlations controlling for the effects of social desirability revealed that difficulty identifying feelings and externally oriented thinking were linked with greater levels of neurocognitive deficits, while difficulty describing feelings was related to heightened levels of emotional distress. To explore whether neurocognition and affective symptoms were uniquely related to alexithymia, a multiple regression was conducted in which neurocognitive scores and affective symptoms were allowed to enter to predict overall levels of alexithymia after controlling for social desirability. Results revealed both processing speed and anxiety uniquely contributed to the prediction of the total score on the TAS-20. Results suggest that dysfunctions in both cognitive and affective processes may be related to alexithymia in schizophrenia independently of one another.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Alexithymia refers to a range of interrelated difficulties, including (a) identifying feelings and distinguishing between feelings and bodily sensations of emotional arousal; (b) describing feelings to others; (c) constricted imaginal processes; and (d) a stimulus-bound, externally orientated cognitive style (Taylor et al., 1997). In contrast to constructs such as mentalization and metacognition, alexithymia focuses specifically on awareness and expression of one׳s own emotions. Heightened levels of alexithymia in a number of different schizophrenia samples (Stanghellini and Ricca, 1995, Cedro et al., 2001, Todarello et al., 2005, Van’t Wout et al., 2007, van der Meer et al., 2009, Koelkebeck et al., 2010 and Kubota et al., 2011) have led several authors to hypothesize that alexithymia may be a vulnerability factor for the development of schizophrenia (Van’t Wout et al., 2007 and van der Meer et al., 2009), and more specifically may be an underlying cause of social dysfunction (Van’t Wout et al., 2007). A range of studies has sought to better understand the presence of alexithymia among persons with schizophrenia by examining its links to other features of illness, particularly symptoms. Stanghellini and Ricca (1995) found that patients with a predominantly non-paranoid, negative symptom presentation were significantly more likely to be identified as alexithymic compared to those presenting primarily with paranoia, whereas Van’t Wout et al. (2007) found that negative symptoms were linked with deficits in identifying feelings only. These authors also noted greater alexithymia among male patients and among male first-degree relatives compared to control subjects. Examining alexithymia and positive symptoms, Cedro et al. (2001) found alexithymia to be more prevalent in persons with the paranoid subtype of schizophrenia compared to matched healthy controls. Other findings challenge the link between alexithymia and positive and negative symptoms. For instance, one study found that negative symptoms abated during the course of one year while alexithymia remained stable (Todarello et al., 2005). This study also observed no significant associations between alexithymia and global psychopathology, positive symptoms, or depression. Similar to others, van der Meer et al. (2009) again found that persons with schizophrenia experienced more difficulties identifying their feelings than others without psychosis, and noted that alexithymia was positively correlated with depression. Gender differences did not emerge as significant in this study. As a whole, this body of work is equivocal and at best raises the possibility of a very complicated relationship between alexithymia and symptoms. Research has to our knowledge, however, yet to examine neurocognition and alexithymia simultaneously in schizophrenia. Neurocognitive deficits have been commonly observed in schizophrenia (Saykin et al., 1991), and there are several reasons to think that they may be linked to alexithymia. Neurobiological research suggests that persons with alexithymia have impaired interhemispheric communication and may have brain structures or functional circuits that are dysfunctional or deficient (Larsen et al., 2003, Taylor and Bagby, 2004 and Wingbermühle et al., 2012). Lane (2008) also proposes a neural model of alexithymia that has some empirical support. In a nonclinical population, higher levels of alexithymia have been associated with poorer neurocognitive test performance, specifically poorer memory and nonverbal intelligence (Onor et al., 2010). Among asymptomatic individuals with HIV, greater alexithymia has been significantly related to poorer attention, working memory, spatial reasoning, and visuospatial organization (Bogdanova et al., 2010). Research on synthetic metacognitive capacity, or the ability to form complex representations of oneself and others, has found that unawareness of one's emotions in schizophrenia is related to poorer verbal memory, processing speed, and executive functioning (Lysaker et al., 2011b), and those who lack the ability to distinguish their own thoughts and feelings tend to have poorer working memory (Lysaker et al., 2007). An improved understanding of the links between alexithymia and neurocognition seems essential if we are to better understand how alexithymia may develop and how it may be addressed in treatment, which appears indicated given its associations with functional impairments (Vanheule et al., 2010) and poor psychotherapeutic outcomes (Ogrodniczuk et al., 2011). To address this issue the current study has examined the relationship of concurrent assessments of alexithymia, neurocognition, and symptoms among a sample of persons with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Our primary prediction was that greater levels of alexithymia would be related to poorer neurocognition. Of note, we assessed a range of different neurocognitive domains and considered the examination of each specific domain to be exploratory in nature. In regard to symptoms, we predicted that alexithymia would be positively and significantly related to depression and anxiety given repeated findings that alexithymia is related to depressive and anxiety disorders (e.g., Parker et al., 1993, Honkalampi et al., 1999, Marchesi et al., 2000, Marchesi et al., 2005, Saarijarvi et al., 2001 and Leweke et al., 2011). We did not anticipate that alexithymia would be significantly related to positive and negative symptoms given the equivocal findings of previous studies. Of note, to assess alexithymia we used the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20; Bagby et al., 1994a). While this is the most utilized measure of alexithymia (Lumley, 2000 and Bagby et al., 2006), it is possible that scores would be biased by socially desirable responding given that the TAS-20 is a self-report measure. Helmes et al. (2008), for instance, found that among three samples of undergraduate students, alexithymia as measured by the TAS-20 was significantly and negatively related to socially desirable responding. In particular, lower levels of alexithymia were significantly related to higher scores on a measure of social desirability that taps unintentional, or unconscious, attempts to portray oneself in a positive light. Given these findings and general concerns about self-report, we included a measure of social desirability to be used as a covariate in other analyses if necessary. Finding Helmes et al. (2008) distinction between intentional and unintentional socially desirable responding interesting, we utilized a two-factor model of the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS; Crowne and Marlowe, 1960) that allowed for similar distinctions.