دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 34393
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

بررسی تاثیر اختلالات فکری و روانی، تعصبات خصومت و پردازش خودکار بر روی نظریه مجرم جنایی ذهن

عنوان انگلیسی
Examining the influence of psychopathy, hostility biases, and automatic processing on criminal offenders' Theory of Mind
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
34393 2015 8 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, Volume 38, January–February 2015, Pages 92–99

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
اختلالات فکری و روانی - تئوری ذهن - همدلی شناختی - تعصب خصومت - پردازش اطلاعات اجتماعی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Psychopathy; Theory of Mind; Cognitive empathy; Hostility bias; Social information processing
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  بررسی تاثیر اختلالات فکری و روانی، تعصبات خصومت و پردازش خودکار بر روی نظریه مجرم جنایی ذهن

چکیده انگلیسی

Theory of Mind (ToM) is a social perceptual skill that refers to the ability to take someone else's perspective and infer what others think. The current study examined the effect of potential hostility biases, as well as controlled (slow) versus automatic (fast) processing on ToM performance in psychopathy. ToM abilities (as assessed with the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test; RMET; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001), was compared between 39 PCL-R diagnosed psychopathic offenders, 37 non-psychopathic offenders, and 26 nonoffender controls. Contrary to our hypothesis, psychopathic individuals presented with intact overall RMET performance when restrictions were imposed on how long task stimuli could be processed. In addition, psychopaths did not over-ascribe hostility to task stimuli (i.e., lack of hostility bias). However, there was a significant three-way interaction between hostility, processing speed, and psychopathy: when there was no time limit on stimulus presentation, psychopathic offenders made fewer errors in identifying more hostile eye stimuli compared to nonoffender controls, who seemed to be less accurate in detecting hostility. Psychopaths' more realistic appraisal of others' malevolent mental states is discussed in the light of theories that stress its potential adaptive function.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Psychopathy is a developmental disorder that is characterized by high levels of antisocial behavior, as well as emotional impairments such as callousness and a lack of moral emotions like remorse (Cleckley, 1941 and Hare, 2003). The disorder is typically assessed using Hare's Psychopathy Checklist Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003). Research has shown this extensively validated instrument to be comprised of two factors (Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988): Factor 1 describes affective and interpersonal items (e.g., shallow affect, conning/manipulative behavior), whereas Factor 2 reflects impulsive and antisocial lifestyle traits (e.g., parasitic lifestyle, irresponsibility).1 A very prominent deficit in psychopathic individuals is their lack of empathy (Cleckley, 1941 and Hare, 2003), a moral emotion that is believed to inhibit antisocial behavior and promote pro-social behavior (Hoffman, 2000). Empathy is usually defined as the capacity to understand and to some extent share the feelings of another person. A distinction is made between at least two forms of empathy, i.e., cognitive and emotional empathy (Feshbach, 1975). Cognitive empathy refers to the ability to take someone else's perspective, and is closely related, or even synonymous to Theory of Mind (ToM). ToM has been described as the capacity to attribute mental states (e.g., intentions, beliefs, and desires) to others (Premack & Woodruff, 1978). In contrast, emotional empathy equals the ability to be responsive to and share in the emotional state of another person (Blair, 2005). Research has shown psychopathic individuals to present with notable emotional empathic deficiencies, like a reduced physiological responsiveness to others' distress (Blair, Jones, Clark, & Smith, 1997). Results of studies on cognitive empathy and psychopathy have been a lot more equivocal. For many years, it has been assumed that adult psychopathy is not associated with ToM deficiencies. A study supporting this supposition was conducted by Blair et al. (1996), who did not find performance differences between psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders on Happé's advanced test of ToM (Happé, 1994), a test that requires subjects to infer story characters' thoughts, feelings, and intentions. Subsequently, Richell et al. (2003) could also not find deficits in psychopathic offenders' ToM using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001), in which subjects are instructed to identify mental states from photographs of the eye region only. Although seemingly supportive of intact ToM in psychopathy, the results of the studies described above have to be interpreted in the light of some limitations. First, sample sizes in the aforementioned studies were relatively small. Second, no previous ToM research has considered the potential influence of automatic versus controlled processing. Taking this distinction into account could be important as information is thought to be processed via two interacting, yet separable neural routes: an affective, subcortical pathway (depending on limbic structures like the amygdala) that provides a ‘quick and dirty’ impression; and a slower, cortical route, which is thought to be responsible for deliberate, cognitive processing, providing a more fine-grained, complex interpretation of information (Adolphs, 2002 and Johnson, 2005). Imaging research on the RMET suggests that amygdala activation mediates performance on this task in healthy individuals (Baron-Cohen et al., 1999). In addition, patients with acquired bilateral amygdala damage have been found to show impairments on the RMET (Stone, Baron-Cohen, Calder, Keane, & Young, 2003). As psychopathy is associated with amygdala dysfunction, yet does not seem to influence RMET performance, it has been suggested that psychopathic people might compensate for their amygdala dysfunction by using cortical brain regions in the identification of mental states (Richell et al., 2003). Possibly, previous studies could not reveal any psychopathy-specific deficits in ToM as subjects could look at task stimuli for as long as they wanted, enabling them to rely on such compensatory cognitive strategies. Another factor that has been overlooked in previous research is the potential influence of offenders' cognitive processing style on ToM performance. Anger and violent behavior are common characteristics in forensic samples, and individuals high on these traits show difficulty ignoring hostile stimuli (Cohen et al., 1998 and Smith and Waterman, 2004). Moreover, both aggression and psychopathy have been found to relate to the presence of hostile attributional biases, i.e., the more elevated these traits are in individuals, the more hostile intent is perceived from others in ambiguous situations (Dodge, 1980 and Vitale et al., 2005). Possibly, preferential attending to hostile stimuli and the incorrect attribution of malevolent intentions to others, could interfere with correctly inferring others' mental states in psychopathy. The current study aimed to build on previous research on ToM by administering the RMET to a group of offenders (varying in their degree of psychopathy) and a group of non-offender subjects. We took a number of precautions in order to overcome the aforementioned limitations. First, we took the possibility into consideration that psychopathic people only present with ToM deficits under conditions where stimuli have to be processed fast. In order to examine this prediction, we presented half of the RMET stimuli rapidly, whereas subjects could look at the other half of the task stimuli without any time restrictions. We expected psychopathic individuals to show impairments on the RMET in comparison with nonoffenders, yet only when stimuli were presented quickly (Hypothesis 1). Second, we investigated whether offenders' ToM performance might be distorted by a cognitive 'hostility bias.' In order to test this hypothesis, we changed a number of incorrect answering options in the RMET, making sure that the alternatives subjects could choose from varied in their degree of hostility. We predicted offenders' performance on the RMET to be distorted by a bias towards more hostile answering options, reflected by a tendency to choose for more hostile answering alternatives when making mistakes (Hypothesis 2). As psychopathy has been found to positively relate to the perception of hostility (Vitale et al., 2005), we predicted this effect to be strongest in the psychopathic offender group, as compared with the nonoffenders.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

The current study aimed to investigate whether psychopathy is related to ToM deficiencies under certain conditions, by comparing (non)psychopathic offenders to nonoffender controls. Contrary to our expectations, results revealed psychopaths' ToM to be intact, even under very stringent conditions (i.e., brief stimulus presentation) or in the presence of hostile answering alternatives, lending further support for intact ToM in psychopathic individuals. An unexpected intriguing finding was that during the long RMET stimulus presentation, psychopathic offenders' performance was positively influenced by the level of hostility in eyes in comparison to nonoffender controls, who seemed to be less accurate in detecting hostility. This finding requires replication though, especially since it is based on a three-way interaction, and constitutes an interesting avenue for future research. These current results should be interpreted in the light of some limitations. First, when using a more stringent cut-off (i.e., a PCL-R score of ≥ 30), our sample included only 19 psychopathic offenders. However, we believe that the inclusion of nonoffender controls, as well as nonpsychopathic offenders made our sample broadly representative. Second, our experimental set-up might not have been appropriate to test whether psychopaths' ToM relies on compensatory cognitive strategies associated with a slower processing speed. Research investigating the social perceptual skills of psychopathic people using neuroimaging techniques might provide more definitive answers to this question. A last drawback of most past research on ToM and psychopathology, including the current study, is that it has disregarded the possibility that individuals' ToM could mainly be distorted in more personally relevant situations, such as when making inferences about what other people think and feel about one personally.