دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 33215
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

کمرویی یک شرط لازم اما نه کافی برای کورتیزول بزاق در کودکان نوعا در حال رشد 10 ساله است

عنوان انگلیسی
Shyness is a necessary but not sufficient condition for high salivary cortisol in typically developing 10 year-old children
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
33215 2007 11 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Personality and Individual Differences, Volume 43, Issue 6, October 2007, Pages 1541–1551

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
کمرویی - کورتیزول بزاقی - ضربان قلب - کودکان
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Shyness; Salivary cortisol; Heart rate; Children
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  کمرویی یک شرط لازم اما نه کافی برای کورتیزول بزاق در کودکان نوعا در حال رشد 10 ساله است

چکیده انگلیسی

Previous research has noted both high and low baseline cortisol levels in anxious personality styles. We examined the relations among shyness, baseline salivary cortisol, and resting heart rate in a non-clinical sample of 10-year-old children. As predicted, shyness was positively correlated with baseline salivary cortisol and resting heart rate, replicating and extending earlier work with preschool age children to middle childhood. However, post-hoc analyses revealed that the scatterplot of shyness and salivary cortisol was significantly triangular in shape, indicating that shyness was a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for high salivary cortisol at age 10. For children high in shyness, salivary cortisol levels ranged from low to high; for children low in shyness, salivary cortisol levels were low. This triangular pattern was not found for the association between shyness and resting heart rate, suggesting different roles of neuroendocrine and autonomic measures in relation to shyness. The potential roles of cortisol in the maintenance of shyness developmentally are discussed as are the broader implications for clarifying prior inconsistent results on the relation between temperament and cortisol in humans. The use of triangular data analytic strategies for interpreting relations between psychological variables and identification of risk and protective factors is also discussed.

مقدمه انگلیسی

The search for psychophysiological and neuroendocrine correlates of temperamental shyness in children has received much attention over the last two decades. Kagan (1994) noted that 10 to 15% of typically developing toddlers who exhibited extreme fear and wariness in response to novelty were likely to be shy and timid as preschoolers. These children he termed “behaviorally inhibited” and noted that this temperamental profile was modestly preserved throughout the early school age years and predictive of psychopathology in late childhood (Kagan & Snidman, 1999). Kagan speculated that the origins of temperamental shyness were linked to individual differences in the excitability of forebrain limbic circuits (particularly the amygdala and its central nucleus) involved in fear regulation. Most recently, Kagan’s group found empirical support for this idea (Schwartz, Wright, Shin, Kagan, & Rauch, 2003). Young adults, who some 20 years earlier were classified as behaviorally inhibited toddlers, exhibited increased amygdalar activity as reflected in fMRI measures in response to novel faces. Kagan and his colleagues earlier noted that temperamentally shy and behaviorally inhibited children exhibited higher morning basal cortisol levels and higher and more stable heart rates compared with uninhibited children (Kagan et al., 1987 and Kagan et al., 1988). We replicated the morning basal cortisol finding in temperamentally shy preschoolers at age four (Schmidt et al., 1997) and noted increased cortisol reactivity in shy children at age seven in response to self-presentation (Schmidt et al., 1999b). However, we did not find high morning basal cortisol levels at age seven in temperamentally shy children (Schmidt, Fox, Schulkin, & Gold, 1999a). Other researchers have noted elevated adrenocortical activity in infants whose temperamental dispositions may predict temperamental shyness in early childhood. For example, Gunnar and her colleagues reported that human infants who remained temperamentally distressed from 9 to 13 months of age exhibited increases in adrenocortical activity during the presentation of laboratory vignettes (Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Larson, & Hertsgaard, 1989). Gunnar’s group also noted that behaviorally inhibited children who were in insecure attachment relationships exhibited greater increases in salivary cortisol during fear-eliciting conditions compared with uninhibited and securely attached children (Nachmias, Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Parritz, & Buss, 1996). More recently, Gunnar and her colleagues found high cortisol levels among 6- to 12-year old children who spent more than 8 months of deprivation in Romanian orphanages in their first years of life compared with early adopted children and Canadian children (Gunnar, Morison, Chisholm, & Schuder, 2001). Despite these numerous findings reviewed above, the relation between high cortisol and stress-anxiety profiles is inconsistent. Cortisol appears to play different roles in a number of temperament styles and even within anxiety profiles. For example, high cortisol levels have been linked to the other end of the temperament spectrum (see Gunnar, 1994, for a review). High cortisol levels have been observed in dominant, assertive, and socially competent children during the preschool and early school years (Granger et al., 1994 and Hart et al., 1995). Higher cortisol levels have been found in dominant monkeys during the formation of social groups compared with subordinate monkeys (Manoque, Leshner, & Candland, 1975). Still other studies have shown that low, not high, cortisol levels characterize some stressed populations (Gunnar & Valdez, 2001). For example, individuals exposed to severe maltreatment (for a review, see Gunnar, 2000), post-traumatic stress disorder (Yehuda et al., 1991 and Yehuda et al., 1995), and stress-related bodily disorders (Heim, Ehlert, & Hellhammer, 2000) exhibit low cortisol levels. As well, we have, mostly recently, found a relation between low baseline salivary cortisol levels and shyness and social phobia in young adults (Beaton et al., 2006). Although there has been considerable attention over the last two decades directed towards identifying biological correlates of temperamental shyness in children, much of this work has focused on the study of infants, preschoolers, and early school age children and adults (see Schmidt & Schulkin, 1999). We know, however, comparatively little regarding temperamental shyness during the middle to late childhood years even though this age point coincides with an important developmental milestone: the onset of puberty and the transition into an adolescent self-concept. The primary purpose of the present study was to extend the relation between temperamental shyness and baseline salivary cortisol and resting heart rate measures noted in preschoolers and early school age children to middle childhood. We examined the relations between shyness and two biological measures (i.e., baseline salivary cortisol and resting mean heart rate) in a cross-sectional study of 10 year-olds who were unselected for individual differences in temperament and who were from a homogenous non-clinical sample. We chose to examine salivary cortisol and heart rate because of the extant literature relating these two measures to temperamental shyness and because both measures are relatively non-invasive when administered to children. For this age group, we predicted that temperamental shyness would be positively related to baseline salivary cortisol and resting heart rate given that these relations have been noted in a majority of studies of younger age children who are shy.