دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 37329
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

سختی زندگی و رفتار ضد اجتماعی نوجوانان: نقش واکنش سیستم عصبی قلبی اتونوم در مطالعه مسیرهای پیاده روی

عنوان انگلیسی
Early life adversities and adolescent antisocial behavior: The role of cardiac autonomic nervous system reactivity in the TRAILS study
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
37329 2015 10 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Biological Psychology, Volume 110, September 2015, Pages 24–33

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
دوره پیش از تخلیه - آریتمی سینوسی تنفسی - رفتارهای ضد اجتماعی - واکنش - بازیابی - سختی
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Pre-ejection period; Respiratory sinus arrhythmia; Antisocial behavior; Reactivity; Recovery; adversity
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  سختی زندگی و رفتار ضد اجتماعی نوجوانان: نقش واکنش سیستم عصبی قلبی اتونوم در مطالعه مسیرهای پیاده روی

چکیده انگلیسی

Abstract In the current study, the role of pre-ejection period (PEP) and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) was studied in the association between prior adversities and antisocial behavior in adolescence. PEP and RSA task reactivity and recovery to a public speaking task were assessed in adolescents from a longitudinal population-based study (N = 624, Mage = 16.14 years, 49.2% boys). Perinatal adversities were unrelated to antisocial behavior, but experiencing more stressful adversities between age 0 and 15 was associated with antisocial behavior at age 16 in boys with blunted PEP reactivity and smaller PEP differences from rest to recovery. Number of adversities between age 0 and 15 was associated with antisocial behavior in boys with blunted and girls with heightened RSA reactivity and larger PEP differences from rest to recovery. The association between prior adversities and antisocial behavior were small in effect size and depended upon sex and PEP and RSA reactivity and recovery.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Introduction Perinatal (i.e., before, during, and right after birth) and early life adversity – here defined as adversity during childhood and adolescence – have been associated with subsequent antisocial behavior in adolescence (Beck & Shaw, 2005; Hodgins, Kratzer, & McNeil, 2001; Raine, Brennan, & Mednick, 1994; Timmermans, van Lier, & Koot, 2010). One of the possible mechanisms underlying this association is the functioning of one of the major stress axes, the cardiac autonomic nervous system (ANS). It has been argued that perinatal adversities shape ANS functioning (Cohen, Vella, Jeffery, Lagercrantz, & Katz-Salamon, 2008; Jones et al., 2008; Kajantie & Raikkonen, 2010). In turn, these changes in ANS functioning may co- occur with changes in emotional and behavioral reactions to environmental stressors and may therefore be associated with the development of antisocial behavior (Ortiz & Raine, 2004; Lorber, 2004). Moreover, adversities during childhood and adolescence have been shown to interact with cardiac ANS functioning (El-Sheikh, Keller, & Erath, 2007; Erath, El-Sheikh, & Cummings, 2009; Gordis, Feres, Olezeski, Rabkin, & Trickett, 2010; Shenk, Noll, Putnam, & Trickett, 2010). In particular, these studies showed that those who experienced adversities during childhood and adolescence and showed blunted or heightened ANS functioning were at increased risk of antisocial behavior. In the current study, we examined these two processes more closely; details and directions of these two mechanisms will be discussed below. 1.1. Autonomic nervous system The cardiac ANS consists of two branches, namely the parasympathetic (PNS) and sympathetic (SNS), which can be assessed by Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and pre-ejection period (PEP), respectively (Alkon et al., 2003; Cacioppo, Tassinary, & Berntson, 2007; Camm et al., 1996). RSA is the heart rate variability due to respiratory gating of tonic vagal effects on the SA node of the heart (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, 1993), and is considered a measure of cardiac vagal control. PEP is derived as the time interval between the onset of ventricular depolarization and the opening of the semilunar valves (Sherwood et al., 1990) and commonly used as an index of myocardial contractility and sympathetic control of the heart (Berntson et al., 1994; Schachinger, Weinbacher, Kiss, Ritz, & Langewitz, 2001). These cardiac ANS measures are used as indicators of the complex processes that underlie autonomic responsiveness to a changing environment, e.g., from rest to a challenge or vice versa. ANS resting measures reflect a physiology state during a calm state. Challenging measures reflect physiological responses to a stressor, and stress reactivity measures indicate physiological responses to a stressor compared to a resting state, whereas recovery measures indicate physiological responses after a stressor compared to a subsequent resting state (Cacioppo et al., 2007). 1.2. Perinatal adversities and antisocial behavior As suggested by evolutionary models of biobehavioral change, perinatal adversities may impact ANS reactivity and recovery because the plasticity of the ANS is strongest early in life (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Del Giudice, Ellis, & Shirtcliff, 2011; Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long, & Griggs, 2009). Several studies support this idea by showing that perinatal adversity has an effect on cardiac ANS reactivity (Enlow et al., 2009 and Jones et al., 2008; Kajantie & Raikkonen, 2010). Based on these prior findings, alterations due to perinatal adversities may be expected to lead to heightened or blunted ANS reactivity in response to a stressor. In turn, empirical studies associating both PNS and SNS reactivity to antisocial behavior showed that reactivity of both branches of the ANS has been cross-sectionally associated with more antisocial behavior (i.e., aggression and externalizing behaviors) in children and adolescents (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007; Boyce et al., 2001; Calkins, Graziano, & Keane, 2007; Sijtsema, Shoulberg, & Murray-Close, 2011). There is theoretical support for both directions, but previous research has shown more support for perinatal and early life adversities being associated with increased ANS reactivity (see Obradovic, 2013 for a discussion). However, both from a theoretical and empirical viewpoint there is reason to believe that the role ANS reactivity plays in the association between adversity and antisocial behavior is different in boys and girls. Evolutionary theorists have argued that in stressful environments it is more adaptive for females to be vigilant and attentive to environmental cues and thus show heightened ANS reactivity, with more internalizing problems as a result (Del Giudice, Hinnant, Ellis, & El-Sheikh, 2012; Glover & Hill, 2012). In contrast, in males it may be more important to respond less to environmental cues, as they are more programmed towards exploring and competing with other males, which may result in antisocial behaviors (Del Giudice et al., 2011). Previous studies investigating sex differences have indeed shown that the association between adverse environments and antisocial behavior was highest in boys with blunted ANS reactivity (Beauchaine, Hong, & Marsh, 2008; Sijtsema et al., 2013), whereas this was not the case in girls. Moreover, a recent study showed that boys high on externalizing problems showed blunted RSA withdrawal, whereas girls high on externalizing problems showed heightened RSA withdrawal (Hinnant, & El-Sheikh, 2013). In sum, perinatal adversities have been found to be associated with both ANS reactivity and antisocial behavior. However, whether ANS reactivity mediates the association between perinatal adversities and antisocial behavior is currently unknown. The first aim of the current study is to test this indirect effect. Because previous studies suggest important sex differences, we hypothesized that blunted ANS reactivity in boys and heightened ANS reactivity in girls mediates the association between perinatal adversities and antisocial behavior at age 16. 1.3. Adversities in childhood and adolescence and antisocial behavior Adversities that take place later in life are less likely to impact alterations in ANS reactivity regulation mechanisms, due to decreased plasticity of the ANS (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Del Giudice et al., 2011 and Gunnar et al., 2009). However, ANS reactivity may modify the association between adversities and antisocial behavior. Moderation by RSA reactivity of the relationship between adversities and antisocial behavior may rest on the premises that RSA withdrawal in response to stress (i.e., removing vagal control) is related to attention and emotional processing (Beauchaine, 2001 and Porges, 1995). Arguably, RSA withdrawal in stressful situations reflects the ability to use attention and emotional strategies to form an appropriate reaction to stress (Bornstein, & Suess, 2000; Porges, 1995). Blunted physiological responses to stress could thus indicate inability to respond adequately to stressful situations. Moderation by PEP reactivity has been studied less frequently but may be related to the behavioral activation system (Brenner, Beauchaine, & Sylvers, 2005). As such, stressful situations or adversity may induce SNS reactivity and blunted reactivity may indicate inability to respond adequately to stressful situations (Beauchaine, 2001). Research into the interaction between context and RSA and PEP reactivity has shown important links to antisocial behaviors (El-Sheikh, Erath, & Hinnant, 2011; El-Sheikh and Hinnant, 2007; Obradovic, Bush, & Boyce, 2011). Specifically, these studies showed that marital conflict in childhood had a stronger effect on externalizing problems (including antisocial behavior) in youth with blunted SNS and RSA reactivity. However, although marital conflict is an important stressor in childhood, these studies did not specifically examine adverse events (e.g., death/illness of a parent or divorce) and whether youth perceived these events as stressful. Moreover, previous studies largely focused on childhood behaviors and did not include adolescent behavior. In addition, most of the studies discussed above have focused on physiological reactivity from rest to stress, but largely ignored recovery from a stressor. Recovery measures are meaningful as healthy individuals in general show elevated physiological activity to stress, but these levels typically decreases relatively quick after the stressor has passed or after habituation to the stressor (cf. Koolhaas et al., 2011). When this natural recovery process is less effective, physiological activation may remain high, even after the stressor has passed. Based on models on allostatic load, chronic or severe stress may lead to a ‘wear and tear’ of the ANS and hence recovery from a stressor takes longer (McEwen, 2007). ANS recovery measures may thus prove an important index for how well individuals are able to regulate their emotions or adapt to their environment after a stressor has passed. However, there is little empirical evidence regarding cardiovascular recovery and some evidence seems to go against theories of prolonged ANS activity during a stressful situation in individuals who experienced more adversity. Research on chronic stress showed that adults with greater chronic stress showed greater systolic blood pressure recovery and higher cortisol levels, with no differences between males and females (Chatkoff, Maier, & Klein, 2010). Similarly, adults who were highest on anticipatory stress, showed the greatest recovery in blood pressure and cortisol (Juster et al., 2012), though another study in adolescents showed no associations between anticipatory stress and ANS recovery (Oldehinkel et al., 2011). We aim to extend previous research by examining whether ANS recovery moderates the association between early life adversities and adolescent antisocial behavior similar to ANS task reactivity. Specifically, based on the earlier presented evolutionary perspective on stress and sex, we hypothesized that in boys, blunted ANS reactivity exacerbates the association between early life adversities and antisocial behavior at age 16, whereas in girls, heightened ANS reactivity exacerbates this association. Similarly, we expected that smaller differences between ANS rest and recovery in boys and larger differences in girls exacerbate the association between early life adversities and antisocial behavior at age 16. We tested these hypotheses separately for number of adversities and the stressfulness of adversities. In addition, we used an ecologically valid experimental condition to induce stress. Childhood adversity and antisocial behavior often pertain to interpersonal stressors, and hence we used an experiment in which ANS reactivity and recovery could be measured in response to an interpersonal stress task (i.e., public speaking task; see Section 2).

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

. Results First, distributions of the variables were checked for approaching normality. The distributions of antisocial behavior were skewed and therefore logarithmically transformed prior statistical analyses. Antisocial behavior was still slightly skewed after transformation (skewness = 1.88). All analyses were performed in IBM SPSS 19.0 and hypotheses were tested two-sidedly using a p-value of <.05 to indicate significance. We also reported marginally significant effects (p < .10), but interpreted these with caution. In Table 1 means, standard deviations, and ranges are reported of all study variables as well as the Spearman correlations between these variables. Boys were significantly higher on antisocial behavior, RSA levels during the speaking task, and dRSAtask compared to girls. No further sex differences were found. Participants who experienced more and more stressful adversities between ages 0 and 15 years were more likely to report antisocial behaviors at age 16. Although perinatal adversities were unrelated to antisocial behavior, participants who experienced more perinatal adversities also experienced more and more stressful adversities between ages 0 and 15 years. Moreover, longer PEP during the speaking task and higher RSA levels during rest, speaking task, and recovery were associated with more antisocial behavior. RSA during speaking task was furthermore associated with having experienced more stressful adversities between age 0 and 15. In contrast, perinatal adversities were negatively associated with PEP duration during speaking task and recovery. Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between all study variables. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 N Mean SD 1. Sex (1 = boy) – – – – 2. Antisocial behavior at age 16 .23 613 0.22 0.29 3. Number perinatal adversities −.06 .04 617 1.01 1.03 4. SA ages 0–15 (Z) .01 .15 .16 595 0.72 2.63 5. NA ages 0–15 (Z) −.07 .12 .23 .44 621 0.01 2.70 6. PEP during rest (ms) −.05 .04 −.05 −.04 .04 622 122.82 19.15 7. PEP during speaking task (ms) .00 .08 -.10 .02 .07 .54 619 106.97 19.50 8. PEP during recovery (ms) −.04 .03 −.09 −.02 .06 .64 .88 615 116.08 19.15 9. dPEPtask .05 .04 −.06 .06 .03 −.47 .49 .26 617 -15.71 18.60 10. dPEPrecovery .00 −.01 −.05 .04 .02 −.43 .40 .42 .86 613 -6.65 16.26 11. RSA during rest (lnms2) −.02 .08 .02 .08 −.00 −.18 −.12 −.16 .07 .04 612 7.00 1.11 12. RSA during speaking task (lnms2) .18 .17 −03 .11 .00 −.07 .05 −.01 .13 .07 .60 608 6.97 1.04 13. RSA during recovery (lnms2) −.04 .09 .06 .06 −.03 −.17 −.14 −.19 .03 −.01 .82 .59 597 7.14 1.14 14. dRSAtask .17 .06 −.06 −.00 −.03 .12 .16 .15 .05 .03 −.52 .31 −.27 602 0.15 0.89 15. dRSArecovery −.05 −.01 .05 −.03 −.06 .01 −.04 −.05 −.05 −.07 −.27 .01 .34 .40 591 0.14 0.69 Note: Correlations in bold are significant at p < .05; SD = standard deviation; Z = Z-transformed scores; RSA = respiratory sinus arrhythmia; PEP = pre-ejection period; NA = number of adversities; SA = stressfulness adversities between age 0 and 15; dPEPtask = pre-ejection period difference score between rest and task; dPEPrecovery = pre-ejection period difference score between rest and recovery; dRSAtask = respiratory sinus arrhythmia difference score between rest and task; dRSArecovery = respiratory sinus arrhythmia difference score between rest and recovery; more details are given in the Section 2. Table options 4.1. Law of initial values The correlations between rest and difference scores of RSA and PEP allowed us to test whether the Law of Initial Values (LIV) was applicable to our data. Based on Geenen and Van de Vijver (1993), we assumed the LIV would be applicable when −rxd > ryd. In other words, the LIV is applicable when the correlation between the rest assessment and the difference between the rest and task assessment is larger than the correlation between the task assessment and the difference between the rest and task assessment. As can be observed from the correlation matrix in Table 1, this only applied to the RSA task reactivity measures (i.e., −(−.52) > .31). To test whether the presence of the LIV is significant, we conducted a Student’s t- test which showed that RSA rest and task scores were significantly different (t(600) = 2.14, p < .05). Hence, we have adjusted our analyses accordingly and included the rest assessment of RSA as a control in the analyses in which we tested the associations between RSA task reactivity and antisocial behavior. 4.2. Mediation by ANS difference scores To assess the indirect (mediation) effect of perinatal adversities on antisocial behavior via ANS reactivity, we tested three paths: (a) the effect of perinatal adversities on antisocial behavior, (b) the effect of ANS reactivity on antisocial behavior, and (c) the direct effect of perinatal adversities on antisocial behavior (e.g., Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007). Correlations in Table 1 indicate that perinatal adversities were not associated with antisocial behavior, yet indirect effects may still be present (MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007). We tested this via moderated mediation analysis using a bootstrap procedure that produces 5000 random samples and hence accounts for the skewness in antisocial behavior at age 16. We tested whether indirect effects were different for boys and girls as either the result of sex differences in the associations between perinatal adversities and ANS reactivity or between ANS reactivity and antisocial behavior, or both. Models were tested separately for dPEPtask, dPEPrecovery, dRSAtask, and dRSArecovery. Bootstrap analyses yielded no significant indirect effects. 4.3. Moderation analyses To test for moderation effects, regression analyses were performed for each cardiac ANS measure (dPEPtask, dPEPrecovery, dRSAtask, dRSArecovery) separately. The dependent variable was antisocial behavior at age 16. Independent variables were a cardiac ANS measure (dPEPtask, dPEPrecovery, dRSAtask, or dRSArecovery), sex, and an adversity measure (number of adversities or stressfulness of adversity). We started the regression analyses with the full model, which included all main effects and all higher order interactions. Subsequently, non-significant interactions were removed from the model specification, and the regression analysis procedure was performed again. This step-down procedure ended when an exclusive significant interaction or main effect was found (McCullagh & Nelder, 1989). Significant interactions were plotted using simple slope analysis (Aiken & West, 1991) and we calculated which slopes significantly differed from zero. To reduce problems with multicollinearity and to ensure that the values plotted in the figures are accurate representations of the data, independent variables were standardized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 (Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004). 4.4. Moderation by PEP difference scores First, we tested our hypothesis that PEP reactivity and recovery moderates the effect of early life adversities on antisocial behavior. In Table 2 significant two-way interactions are reported between sex and dPEPtask and dPEPrecovery respectively (sr2 = .01 for both interactions) in the models including the number of adversities (NA; model 1 and model 3), suggesting that blunted dPEPtask and dPEPrecovery scores were associated with more antisocial behavior in boys. Model 2 also showed a marginally significant three-way interaction between sex, stressfulness of adversities, and dPEPtask (sr2 = .01). This effect is depicted in Fig. 2. Simple slope analyses indicated that only in boys with more blunted dPEPtask, higher stressfulness of adversities were associated with antisocial behavior (β = .17, p < .05). In girls, the association between the stressfulness of adversities and antisocial behavior was unrelated to dPEPtask. These results (model 2) were similar for dPEPrecovery (model 4). Table 2. Beta-coefficients of the effects of sex, adversities, and pep and rsa difference scores on antisocial behavior. Model Pred1 β Pred2 β Pred3 β Pred4 β Pred5 β Pred6 β dPEPtask 1 (R2 = 10.5%) Sex 0.27 *** NA 0.17 *** Sex* dPEP −0.11 † 2 (R2 = 11.9%) Sex 0.27 *** SA 0.13 * Sex* dPEP −0.12 * Sex*SA*dPEP −0.10 † dPEPrecovery 3 (R2 = 10.7%) Sex 0.27 *** NA 0.17 *** Sex* dPEP −0.17 ** 4 (R2 = 12.5%) Sex 0.27 *** SA 0.13 * Sex* dPEP −0.16 ** Sex*SA*dPEP −0.10 † dRSAtask 5 (R2 = 10.4%) Sex 0.21 *** NA 0.19 *** RSAr 0.13 * Sex*dRSA −0.10 * dRSA* NA 0.12 * Sex*dRSA*NA −0.08 † 6 (R2 = 9.1%) Sex 0.20 *** SA 0.15 † RSAr 0.11 * Sex*dRSA −0.12 * dRSArecovery 7 (R2 = 9.3%) Sex 0.22 *** NA 0.16 ** dRSA*NA 0.15 ** Sex*dRSA*NA −0.12 * 8 (R2 = 7.7%) Sex 0.22 *** SA 0.12 ** Pred = predictor; NA = number of adversities between age 0 and 15; SA = stressfulness adversities between age 0 and 15; dPEPtask = pre-ejection period difference score between rest and task; dPEPrecovery = pre-ejection period difference score between rest and recovery; RSAr = respiratory sinus arrhythmia rest score; dRSAtask = respiratory sinus arrhythmia difference score between rest and task; dRSArecovery = respiratory sinus arrhythmia difference score between rest and recovery; more details are given in Section 2; see Supplementary online appendices for the full tables, including B(SE), 95% confidence intervals, and squared semipartial correlations for all parameters. † p < 0.10. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001. Table options Simple slopes of number of adversities between ages 0 and 15 on antisocial ... Fig. 2. Simple slopes of number of adversities between ages 0 and 15 on antisocial behavior at age 16 plotted at −1 and +1 standard deviation of dPEPtask (model 2/4 in Table 2) for boys and girls. Figure options 4.5. Moderation by RSA difference scores Next, we tested our hypothesis that RSA reactivity moderates the effect of early life adversities on antisocial behavior. There was a marginally significant interaction between sex, dRSAtask, and number of adversities between ages 0 and 15 years (sr2 < .01; model 5). However, simple slope analysis ( Fig. 3A) indicated that the separate slopes for boys were significantly different from zero. That is, boys with more blunted dRSAtask and a higher number of adversities were more likely to report antisocial behavior than boys with heightened dRSAtask (β = .27, p < .01 and β = .18, p < .05, respectively). In girls, the association between number of adversities between ages 0 and 15 years was stronger for those with heightened dRSAtask (1SD above average; β = .29, p < .001) compared to those with blunted dRSAtask (1SD below average; β = .07, n.s.). These marginally significant three-way interactions were in line with the significant two-way interactions in model 5. There were no significant interactions between dRSAtask and stressfulness of adversities (see model 6). A and B: sImple slopes of number of adversities between ages 0 and 15 on ... Fig. 3. A and B: sImple slopes of number of adversities between ages 0 and 15 on antisocial behavior at age 16 plotted at −1 and +1 standard deviation of dRSAtask (A, model 5 in Table 2) and dRSArecovery (B, model 7 in Table 2) for boys and girls. Figure options In Model 7 a significant three-way interaction was found with regard to dRSArecovery (sr2 = .01; see Fig. 3B), which can be interpreted in a similar way. The number of adversities was more strongly associated with antisocial behavior in girls with heightened dRSArecovery (β = .31, p < .01) compared to girls with blunted dRSArecovery. In boys, experiencing more adversities between age 0 and 15 was associated with reporting more antisocial behavior, irrespective of dRSArecovery (β = .28, p < .01 for low dRSArecovery and β = .16, p = .10 for high [1SD above average] dRSArecovery). There were no significant interactions between dRSArecovery and stressfulness of adversities (see model 8).