فراموشی عمدی انتخابی در نوجوانان مبتلا به اختلال اضطراب اجتماعی
کد مقاله | سال انتشار | تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی |
---|---|---|
39206 | 2013 | 5 صفحه PDF |
Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)
Journal : Psychiatry Research, Volume 208, Issue 2, 30 July 2013, Pages 151–155
چکیده انگلیسی
Abstract Anxiety in young adults has recently been linked to reduced capacities to inhibit the processing of non-affective perceptual distractors. However, no previous research has addressed the relationship between social anxiety disorder (SAD) and the ability to intentionally inhibit no longer relevant memories. In an experimental study with adolescents diagnosed with SAD and matched nonclinical controls, a selective directed forgetting procedure was used to assess the extent to which anxious individuals showed lower memory impairment for to-be-forgotten information than their non-anxious counterparts. The results revealed that while the nonclinical sample group demonstrated the ability to selectively forget when instructed, the anxious adolescents demonstrated good memory for to-be-forgotten material and therefore failed to forget. Interestingly, more severe SAD symptomatology inversely predicted a degree of forgetting. We conclude that the main difference between socially anxious and non-anxious participants is specifically related to the ability to intentionally forget and could reflect cognitive functioning that is associated with vulnerability to anxiety. Impairment of the ability to make unwanted memories less retrievable could prompt some individuals to initiate or maintain anxiety disorders. Future psychological treatments could benefit from including modules on memory control training.
مقدمه انگلیسی
Introduction Over the last 25 years, research on anxiety has focused on characterizing its underlying cognitive functioning (MacLeod et al., 1986, Cloitre and Liebowitz, 1991, Beck and Clark, 1997, Bishop, 2009 and Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2010). Thus, influencing theories on anxiety such as those by Eysenck (1992) or Beck and Clark (1997) stress the presence of hyper-vigilance and cognitive biases in anxiety disorders. In this respect, some studies comparing people with anxiety disorders and control individuals suggest that anxiety entails attentional and memory biases for threat-related stimuli. Thus, for example, it has been shown that patients with generalized anxiety disorder have greater difficulty than control participants in disengaging attention from task-irrelevant information related to threat (Williams et al., 1996). Hence, it is not surprising that a leading theoretical stance states that anxiety is characterized by a hyper-vigilant threat-detection system (Mathews et al., 1997). More recently, anxiety has been related to reduced executive control capacity (Airaksinen et al., 2005, Bishop, 2009 and Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2010). Executive (or cognitive) control refers to a variety of goal-oriented processes that regulate thought, action and emotion. Thus, for example, it has been proposed that executive control processes play a key role in overcoming conflicts1 that occur during information processing (Botvinick et al., 2001). Evidence of the link between anxiety and executive control comes from studies wherein anxious people tend to show deficits across a variety of non-affective tasks that demand high levels of cognitive control (Fox, 1993, Bishop, 2009 and Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2010). For example, in a neuroimaging study, Bishop (2009) found that high-trait-anxiety individuals showed reduced recruitment of the control mechanisms in charge of inhibiting distracting information that competes for processing resources. In a similar vein, Pacheco-Unguetti et al. (2010) studied the efficiency of executive control processes in high-trait-anxiety and low-trait-anxiety individuals. By using an experimental procedure thought to draw on cognitive control (Callejas et al., 2004), the authors obtained measures of the amount of interference experienced by participants while stimulation conflicting with targets was presented. It was assumed that more interference from distracting information stems from less efficient executive control mechanisms. Pacheco-Unguetti et al. showed that high-trait-anxiety individuals found it more difficult to control interference from distractors than their low-trait-anxiety counterparts. Interestingly, no such deficit in executive control was found when a high-state-anxiety group was compared with a low-state-anxiety group in the same task. Taken together, results by Bishop and Pacheco-Unguetti et al. suggest that trait anxiety entails an impoverished capacity to exert control over distracting information (i.e., by inhibiting it). Maybe more importantly, as this deficit was observed when the processing of non-affective information was required, it could be thought of as a general characteristic of trait anxiety rather than as a threat-based cognitive bias. Executive control is also involved in controlling memory (Conway et al., 2000, Braver and Cohen, 2001, Anderson, 2003, Román et al., 2009 and Anderson and Huddleston, 2011). Specifically, it has been put forward that updating memory contents may require an inhibitory mechanism in charge of rendering non-relevant memories less accessible (Bjork, 1998 and Anderson, 2005). Results in experiments using the list method-directed forgetting2 (LM-DF) procedure, for example, might be interpreted in terms of inhibitory executive control (Conway et al., 2000 and Anderson, 2005). In a standard LM-DF experiment, participants are presented with a first list of items to study. Immediately after being presented with List 1, half of the participants are instructed to forget the whole list (forget group), whereas the remaining participants are told to continue remembering List 1 (remember group). After being provided with a second list for study, both groups perform a recall test that usually involves both studied lists. The typical DF effect is a lower recall for List 1 items in the forget group relative to the remember group. From an inhibitory framework, this memory impairment might reflect the after-effect of an inhibitory control mechanism that acts on List 1. Specifically, it has been suggested that the instruction to forget in the presence of two sets of information induces a temporary state of inhibition that makes to-be-forgotten items less retrievable in a subsequent recall test (Bjork, 1998 and Pastötter and Bäuml, 2010). A better understanding of memory control capacities in anxiety disorders may be relevant given that some theoretical models (more or less implicitly) highlight the role of memory retrieval in anxiety. Thus, for example, in a cognitive model of social phobia (Clark and Wells, 1995 and Wells and Matthews, 1996), individuals engage in anticipatory processing concerning past social situations (which necessarily requires episodic retrieval) before coming across a new social event. Hence, LM-DF procedures could serve as marker tasks to assess memory control abilities in anxious people. However, little work has focused on examining the effect of an instruction to forget in people with anxiety disorders. An exception to this is a study by Power et al. (2000) aimed at exploring the ability of clinically depressed, clinically anxious and control individuals to intentionally forget. Unfortunately, the results of this study do not allow us to learn anything about the memory control capacities of anxious people since DF was not even observed in the control group. Hence, the present study aimed to examine the relationship between trait anxiety and memory control by using an LM-DF procedure. Specifically, we utilized a recently introduced variant of this experimental procedure we thought to be more suitable for exploring individual differences in memory control (Delaney et al., 2009). The rationale behind our choice lies on the fact that this new procedure requires participants to selectively forget part of List 1 (rather than forget the whole set of studied items) so that it can draw on higher executive control demands. To the best of our knowledge, there is no previous research addressing the link between selective directed forgetting and a particular anxiety disorder. Besides, while research on the relationship between anxiety and cognition has focused on young adults, studies on this relationship in children and adolescents are sparse (Mesa et al., 2011). To fill this gap, the present study focuses on a sample of clinically diagnosed adolescents with social anxiety disorder (SAD) as part of a broader clinical project. SAD is a disabling disorder characterized by a fear of negative evaluation in social situations, has a lifetime prevalence that usually ranges between 2% and 9% in adolescents (Vasa et al., 2007 and Orgiles et al., 2008), and is associated with significant social and academic impairments (Silverman and Albano, 1996). To summarize, we aimed to test the hypothesis that clinically anxious adolescents are less able to selectively forget outdated non-affective memories than non-anxious adolescents. If a structural general-domain deficit in executive processes characterized trait anxiety, one would expect people with anxiety disorders to be less efficient at exerting control over out-of-date memories, even if these episodic memories were not affective in nature.
نتیجه گیری انگلیسی
Results Recalled sentences were considered correct if they kept the gist of the original ones and were assigned to the right character (overall, assignation errors were less than 7%). Two independent coders scored the recall performance of each participant. The interrater agreement was greater than 95%. When agreement was not reached, two additional coders were considered to resolve the differences through discussion. Since our hypotheses only concerned List 1, we only report detailed data analyses for that list. 3.1. Selective directed forgetting (SDF) We first carried out a 2 (anxiety group: Clinical Disorder vs. Control)×2(instruction: Remember vs. Forget)×2(character: Tom vs. Alex) mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) on recall rates. Results showed the higher order interaction to be marginally significant, F(1,160)=3.28, p=0.072, ηp2=0.020, and separate ANOVAs were performed for each anxiety group (see Fig. 1). Mean percentage of sentences correctly recalled as a function of diagnosis ... Fig. 1. Mean percentage of sentences correctly recalled as a function of diagnosis group, List 1 character and instruction provided to participants. Bars indicate standard error of the mean. Note: TBR=Character to be remembered in the forget condition. TBF=Character to be forgotten in the forget condition. In the remember condition both characters were to be remembered. Figure options A 2 (instruction)×2 (character) mixed ANOVA on the nonclinical participants' recall rates showed a significant effect of the interaction, F(1,102)=4.50, p=0.036, ηp2=0.042. Further analyses revealed reliable SDF; the group cued to forget recalled fewer items about Tom than the remember-cued group did, F(1, 102)=4.37, p=0.039, ηp2=0.041, whereas both groups tended to recall the same amount of items about Alex, F(1,102)<1. The corresponding ANOVA on the anxious participants' recall rates, however, did not show significant effects (all with ps>0.220; Instruction×Character interaction with F(1,58)<1). Tom and Alex items were recalled to the same extent regardless of the instruction provided after the List 1 study. Interestingly, this pattern did not seem to be due to the clinical group's unwillingness to forget, as a two-tailed t-test on the responses to the post-experimental Likert-type item revealed that anxious and control participants took the instruction to forget in a similar way (M=1.78, S.D.=2.10 and M=2.06, S.D.=2.10; p=0.50). Moreover, the absence of memory impairment in adolescents with SAD remained the same even after limiting the analyses to those participants who more clearly reported having followed the instruction to forget (values in the Likert-type item >2). Additional analyses confirmed that there were no recall differences between the participants, with and without anxiety disorder, cued to remember all List 1 items, F(1,49)=1.69, p=0.199, ηp2=0.033; (Character×Group interaction with F(1,49)<1). When comparing the two groups cued to forget, however, the interaction Character×Group was significant, F(1,111)=5.32, p=0.022, ηp2=0.046. Whereas the nonclinical group showed a reliable memory impairment for Tom sentences relative to those for Alex, F(1,111)=6.95, p=0.009, ηp2=0.059, this was not the case in the clinical group, which tended to recall sentences about the two characters at the same level, F(1,111)<1. 3.2. Relation between symptomatology and forgetting To further explore the link between anxiety and memory control, we correlated the scores on the CDI and the SAS-A with individuals scores on selective forgetting. This latter variable was calculated for each of the participants with anxiety disorder in the forget condition by subtracting his/her recall rate for the to-be-forgotten character (Tom) from the recall rate for the to-be-remembered one (Alex) and dividing the result by the recall rate of Alex. The analyses showed a marginally significant correlation between the total SAS-A score and the score of selective directed forgetting, r=−0.32, t(31)=−1.79, p=0.083. Additional analyses revealed that the scores in the subscale SAD-G of the SAS-A were the only ones reliably predicting memory control, r=−0.36, t (33)=−2.15, p=0.039. In other words, the higher the level of anxiety and general social avoidance, the lower the ability to selectively forget. Scores on the CDI did not correlate with the measure of forgetting (p=0.60).