دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 33006
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

نوع دوستی نسبت به غریبه ها در مواقع نیاز: سیگنالینگ هزینه در یک جامعه صنعتی

عنوان انگلیسی
Altruism towards strangers in need: costly signaling in an industrial society
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
33006 2010 9 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Evolution and Human Behavior, Volume 31, Issue 2, March 2010, Pages 95–103

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
سخاوت - شناخت اجتماعی - پیشنهادات خیریه -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Generosity; Social recognition; Charity offer
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  نوع دوستی نسبت به غریبه ها در مواقع نیاز: سیگنالینگ هزینه در یک جامعه صنعتی

چکیده انگلیسی

In the present study, the costly signaling theory (CST) is used to examine the effect of an offer of charity on social recognition. On behalf of a charitable organization, 186 students enrolled in 16 different courses were asked to offer support to unfamiliar persons in need. In accordance with our predictions, the results show that significantly more subjects are willing to give assistance if they make charity offers in the presence of their group members than when the offers are made in secret. In accordance with CST—but not with the prevailing explanations in social psychology—the likelihood of charity service was strongly influenced by the expected cost of altruistic behavior. Publicly demonstrated altruistic intentions yielded long-term benefits: Subjects who were willing to participate in a particular charity activity gained significantly higher sociometry scores (as a sign of social recognition) than did others. The cost of volunteerism correlated with social recognition in the case of a charity act judged as the most expensive (giving assistance to mentally retarded children), but not for the other categories of charity offer. Our results suggest that public generosity towards strangers as a costly signal may convey reliable information about subjects' personality traits, such as cooperativeness, but our data do not support the hypothesis that the signaling mechanism is related to sexual selection and mate choice.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Why do people help others who are unfamiliar to them and cannot be expected to return the favor? Generosity seems to be a cross-culturally ubiquitous feature of life. It is well documented that humans are often altruistic toward unrelated individuals, even strangers. Several evolutionary explanations have been provided for explaining generous acts towards non-kin: indirect reciprocity (Alexander, 1987 and Milinski et al., 2001), strong reciprocity (Bowles & Gintis, 2004 and Fehr et al., 2002), reputation-building (Bereczkei et al., 2007, Milinski et al., 2000 and Semmann et al., 2005), competitive altruism (Barclay, 2004 and Hardy & Vugt, 2006), altruistic punishment, etc. (Bernhard et al., 2006 and Boyd et al., 2003). In the light of these theories, generosity does not seem to be an unconditional motive. Instead, it appears to be strategic: the actual situation in which an altruistic act occurs and the personality of both donor and recipient deeply influence behavioral output (Smith & Bliege Bird, 2000 and Smith & Bliege Bird, 2005). 1.1. Costly signaling theory Costly signaling theory (CST) states that generosity is one means by which individuals gain social recognition and preferential treatment in their group and thereby gain reproductive benefits in the long run (Gurven et al., 2000, Bliege Bird, & Hawkes, 2002, Smith & Bliege Bird, 2000, Smith & Bliege Bird, 2005 and Sosis, 2000). Individuals who engage in altruistic acts serve their own interests by reliably demonstrating qualities that underlie the altruistic act, such as resource control, trustworthiness, social skills, etc. For the signal to be reliable, it should be costly in terms of energy, time expenditure, and potential risk (Bliege Bird, & Hawkes, 2002 and Smith, 2000). Public generosity—providing collective goods, charity, donation, etc.—implies especially tremendous costs in terms of physical activity, time, money, etc. This kind of helping behavior is considered an indiscriminate or unconditional altruistic act because it is usually directed to strangers who have no chance of returning the favor. Public generosity as a honest signal may benefit both signaler and observers. The payoff to the altruist comes from being favored by the others as a reliable partner in cooperative relationships and chosen as ally, mate, or competitor (Gurven et al., 2000). Studies with experimental games have revealed that donations to strangers deeply influence the social attitudes of group members toward the altruist (Milinski et al., 2000 and Semmann et al., 2005). The payoff to the observer comes from the usefulness of the information inferred from the altruistic act as a signal. The observer can evaluate the signaler's qualities that would be beneficial in future social interactions in the group, involving friendship, alliance, and mating. In this respect, costly signaling may be regarded as a kind of reputation-gaining strategy. The difference is that CST specifies mechanisms leading to the benefits (social recognition, prestige, preferential treatment) that individuals gain within their groups. The signaling mechanism provides information about the relevant underlying qualities of the altruist (signaler). This information could then favorably alter the behavior of other group members towards the altruist (Smith & Bliege Bird, 2005). CST has been tested in pre-industrial societies: for example, among the Ache of Paraguay (Gurven et al., 2000), in the Ifaluk society (Sosis, 2000), and in the community of the Conambe (Ecuadorian Amazon) (Patton, 2005). Turtle hunting and the associated public feasting among the Meriam of Torres Strait, Australia, is one of the best documented forms of public generosity (Bliege Bird & Bird, 1997, Smith & Bliege Bird, 2000, Smith & Bliege Bird, 2005 and Smith et al., 2003). Although successful male turtle hunters receive no recompense, not even portions of their catch in the subsequent feast, they gain net benefit form their risky endeavor, which is realized through multiple avenues (Gurven, 2004). Those who lead turtle hunts appear to be signaling leadership skills, specialized knowledge and generosity in supplying collective goods, as well as their good physical condition and willingness to take risks, which are highly valued traits in this community. As a possible result, they were found to gain social recognition, have an earlier onset of reproduction, achieve higher age-specific reproductive success, and gain highly ranked mates. The solid empirical foundation of CST comes from the preindustrial societies. Although there have been several investigations on costly signaling applications in contemporary urban communities (Farthing, 2005, Goldberg, 1995 and Lyons, 2005), well-controlled empirical studies have not yet been conducted in modern industrial societies. This failure is especially pressing, if a crucial difference between technically less and more advanced societies is taken into consideration. Studies in preindustrial societies have referred to circumstances where both signaler and observer lived in the same group and both generosity and preferential treatment toward altruists occurred inside the group. However, it is well documented that humans are often altruistic to strangers, especially in industrial societies, for example, in donating to charities. 1.2. Charity service and social recognition Our research is based on a realistic (life-like) situation in which subjects have an opportunity to decide freely whether they wish to help unfamiliar people (Bereczkei et al., 2007). A representative of a charity organization requested students in a seminar course to support unfamiliar people in need (alone, elderly, homeless, and mentally retarded people). In one setting, students could make their offers publicly in the presence of their group, while in another setting, the offers were concealed, so the others in the group were not aware of them. We expected that both altruistic behavior and the subsequent change in social recognition would differ as a function of these circumstances, as follows. Prediction 1. People who can make their charity offers in the presence of group members are more likely to offer support to strangers than those whose intention to provide assistance remains concealed from the group. Prediction 2. Subjects who make their charity offers to the needy in the presence of and with the knowledge of their group members should gain social recognition (reputation, prestige, and popularity) within the group. 1.3. Costs for helping However, these predicted relationships between altruistic acts and social recognition can be interpreted by certain non-evolutionary theories, especially social–psychological explanations. Although the presence of others can inhibit people from responding to an emergency (Latané & Darley, 1970), in general, people are more likely to engage in prosocial behavior when they are observed by others (Schroeder, Penner, Dovidio, & Piliavin, 1995). The presence of others has long been known to stimulate altruism, generosity, and cooperation (Berkowitz, 1972, Eisenberg-Berg, 1979, Hofman et al., 1994, Hogg & Vaughan, 2005 and Jerdee & Rosen, 1974). Studies in evolutionary psychology have also found that an increase in the visibility and decrease in the anonymity of individuals enhances their cooperation in social dilemmas (Haley, 2005 and Kurzban et al., 2007). From this perspective, the visibility of potential altruists can be, in itself, responsible for the higher frequency of a charity offer without the need to assume altruistic signaling and reputation-gaining strategies. However, CST provides unique predictions that cannot be inferred from other psychological theories. It claims that the willingness to provide generosity and the related social recognition will depend on the perceived costs of the altruist act. When being observed, people are likely to engage in the costly acts of public generosity and highly value those persons who choose helping behavior with a high expenditure of time, energy, and risk-taking. In contrary, in light of the prevalent social psychological theories, people are motivated to minimize the costs imposed by helping behavior (effort, time, danger, disruption of ongoing activities, etc.), and they engage in prosocial behavior when immediate reward or direct compensation counterbalance these costs (Dovidio et al., 2006). Researchers have found that personal costs for helping can readily outweigh even the serious need of the victim, regardless of whether others were present or not (Pilavin & Pilavin, 1972). Furthermore, even the factors that are seemingly unimportant compared to the suffering of the victims, such as time pressure, often make people with good personality traits less responsive to the needs of the others, in spite of the fact that they were observed by their group mates (Darley & Batson, 1973). Prediction 3. Whereas the frequency of social support is expected to be inversely proportional to the costs of the charity activity (i.e., more people are willing to provide less costly support than more costly support), this relation is highly context dependent. In accordance with the theory of the costly signaling model, more people offer costly support in groups in which they can make their offers publicly than do people in groups in which the offers are made in secret. In the latter group, less costly offers will tend to be more frequent. Prediction 4. Social recognition as a benefit is directly proportional to the costs of altruistic investment. The social attraction of individuals who make costly charity offers (when others in the group learn about it) will grow more than that of people who make less costly contributions to assist a charity organization. 1.4. Signaling mechanisms 1.4.1. Sexual selection CST can also provide distinctive predictions about the individual characteristics being signaled. One possibility is that public generosity is related to sexual selection whereby men are signaling qualities that increase their chances to get sexual partners and establish long-term partnership. In fact, several studies of pre-industrial societies (the Ache, Meriam, Hadza, etc.) have revealed that young, unmarried men display costly activities associated with sharing their own resources in a way that was generous and also highly desirable for potential mates (Bliege Bird, & Hawkes, 2002, Hawkes, 1991 and Sosis, 2000). In light of evolutionary theory, men who have been selected to compete for resources can achieve a greater fitness return as compared to women through high cost–high return strategies (Geary, 1998 and Mealey, 2000). Thus, public generosity can be seen as a form of competition among men that increases their reproductive success in terms of getting sexual partners. This argument seems to contradict the more traditional view in social sciences stating that women who have more empathy, show more care and are more sociable than men will on average participate in charity activities in greater number than members of the opposite sex—regardless of the condition of publicity. Prediction 5. Costly signaling is more characteristic of men and less characteristic of women. Men are more likely to initiate charity offers publicly, while more women are willing to participate in organized charity anonymously, even if their intention is concealed from the group. 1.4.2. Personality traits Public generosity toward strangers as a costly signal may convey reliable information about subjects' personalities and abilities that might be hidden in everyday interactions. By offering charity service, university students in a seminar class can demonstrate various psychological traits associated with prosocial personalities such as trustworthiness, empathy, and solidarity. These features might refer to an altruist's benevolence and helpfulness toward group members, who may regard these qualities as valuable and useful in future interpersonal relationships. Prediction 6. Individuals who gain an above-average social recognition and are chosen as best friends among their group mates are expected to have a higher prosocial personality than those with lower scores of social recognition.