سبک های دلبستگی و فرزند داری در کودکان در سن مدرسه که لکنت زبان دارند
کد مقاله | سال انتشار | تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی |
---|---|---|
33541 | 2012 | 13 صفحه PDF |
Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)
Journal : Journal of Communication Disorders, Volume 45, Issue 2, March–April 2012, Pages 98–110
چکیده انگلیسی
Parental input has been described as influential in early childhood stuttering yet the exact nature of this influence remains equivocal. The present study aimed to examine whether quantitative measures of parenting styles, parent and peer attachment patterns, and parent- and self-reported child behaviour could differentiate between school-aged children who stutter (CWS) (n = 10) and their fluent peers (n = 10). In addition, qualitative individual semi-structured interviews with all CWS were conducted to gain insight into their life experiences and reflections in relation to stuttering. The interviews were classified into ancillary themes of school, peers and parents. Quantitative findings revealed that CWS perceived their parents with significantly lower attachment, particularly in relation to trust, and parents of CWS perceived their children with significantly higher maladjustments than fluent counterparts. Qualitative themes emerged pertaining to attitudes, perceptions and relationships with teachers, peers and parents, with consistent experiences of teasing and bullying reported as a consequence of the stutter. The majority of participants recounted frustration with the nature in which their parents attempted to remediate their stuttering. Collectively, these findings highlight imperative management considerations for school-aged CWS and their parents. The usefulness of quantitative and qualitative research paradigms is also emphasised. Learning outcomes: The reader will be able to: (1) identify themes associated with the impact a childhood stutter has on parent and peer relationships; (2) identify how the quality of the parent child relationship is influenced by parenting styles and attachment; and (3) discuss the clinical implications of the results for children who stutter and their families.
مقدمه انگلیسی
The development of a stuttering disorder is viewed as complex and multi-factorial, and is increasingly perceived to be influenced by environmental, genetic and constitutional factors (Guitar, 2003 and Yaruss and Quesal, 2006). Parents have been recognised as influencing stuttering in some way; however the nature of this influence remains equivocal. Johnson's (1942) diagnosogenic theory proposed stuttering to be caused by parents’ abnormal reactions to, and inaccurate labelling of normal dysfluencies (Bloodstein & Bernstein Ratner, 2008). As a result, therapy was aimed at modifying the interactions and responses of parents of children who stuttered (CWS) (Siegel, 1998). While this notion of causality is debatable, parental input is often considered an essential component in the amelioration of stuttering (Bernstein Ratner, 2004 and Finn and Cordes, 1997). Speech pathologists working with paediatric clients often rely on parents to actively engage in the therapeutic process, in order to facilitate successful fluency outcomes. 1.1. Parenting styles and parent–child attachment concepts 1.1.1. Parenting styles Parenting style and parent–child attachment (PCA) are two discrete, but closely related concepts. Parenting style refers to patterns of childrearing evolved from parents’ reactions towards their child. Conversely, PCA is concerned with the dyadic emotional bond that emerges between parent and child (Bell and Calkins, 2000 and Desjardins et al., 2008). Parenting style is commonly viewed along two dimensions; responsiveness and demandingness (e.g., Desjardins et al., 2008, Gullone and Robinson, 2005, McGarvey et al., 1999 and Wilhelm et al., 2005). Responsiveness, or care, refers to the degree of warmth between parent and child, while demandingness, or control, refers to the extent of disciplinary practice and behavioural standards parents set for their child ( Carlo, McGinley, Hayes, Batenhorst, & Wilkinson, 2007). Based on these two dimensions, a taxonomy of four main parenting styles has been identified which include: authoritarian (high demandingness, low responsiveness), authoritative (high on both constructs), permissive (low demandingness, high responsiveness) and neglectful (low on both constructs) (e.g., Luther, 2007, McGillicuddy-De Lisi and De Lisi, 2007 and Slicker, 1998). 1.1.2. Parent–child attachment According to Bowlby's (1969) ethological-evolutionary attachment theory (e.g., Bowlby, 1969 and Bowlby, 1970, see also Ainsworth, 1979) human beings possess an adaptive system in which infants seek proximity with their caregivers for survival and protection. When danger recedes, infants explore the environment freely to gain new knowledge and experiences. Over time, an “internal working model” (Leveridge, Stoltenberg, & Beesley, 2005, p. 578) is formed so that the child develops knowledge about self and attachment figures. This model forms the foundation from which the child interprets and anticipates the behaviours of others. This inner representation is thought to be flexible in childhood, but becomes increasingly resistant to change in adulthood (Leveridge et al., 2005). Four main attachment styles, based on infants’ responses to their caregivers, have evolved from Ainsworth's Strange Situation Assessment (Ainsworth et al., 1978 and Ainsworth, 1979). These include: secure, avoidant, ambivalent-resistant and disorganised/disoriented. Secure attachment results when the child is able to use significant attachment figures as a safe base to explore their environment, yet are comforted by physical contact when upset. Avoidant attachment is characterised by the child's lack of distress when separated from the attachment figure. The child also avoids interacting with their caregiver. Ambivalent-resistant infants tend to cry when separated from their caregivers, yet continue to cry and are unlikely to be easily soothed even when comforted. Disorganised/disoriented infants do not respond systematically to their caregiver. These infants tend to seek proximity yet lean away from their attachment figure at the same time ( Ainsworth, 1979, Waters et al., 2000 and Yoo et al., 2006). Motivated by the research into infant attachment, Mary Main and colleagues (1985) addressed adult attachment which led to the development of the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI). The language and coherence of recounted relationships reflected by the adult in the assessment is said to reveal mental representations of attachment experiences (Van Ijzendoorn, 1995). Building on Bowlby's theory, Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) derived a two-dimensional model to systematically describe adult attachment. The two dimensions are models of self (degree of positivity towards self) and models of others (degree of positive towards others), giving rise to four attachment patterns: secure/autonomous, preoccupied, dismissive, and unresolved/disorganised. Autonomous adults view their attachment experiences as influential and have a positive viewpoint of themselves and others. These adults are comfortable with intimacy. Preoccupied attachment results when adults hold positive viewpoints of others, but not of themselves. These individuals are confused about their past experiences and they tend to strive for self-acceptance through continuous attempts to please others such as their parents. Dismissive adults are characterised by positive viewpoints of themselves, however are dismissive of others and tend to avoid intimacy by maintaining independence. Lastly, unresolved or disorganised attachment is characterised by negative viewpoints of both self and others. Such adults are fearful of intimacy and tend to avoid social situations ( Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991, Griffin and Bartholomew, 1994 and Yoo et al., 2006). These terminologies have also been described in Target, Fonagy, and Shmueli-Goetz (2003) Child Attachment Interview, an attachment measure in middle childhood (8–13 years), derived from the AAI. Previous research in stuttering disorders has emphasised facets of PCA, such as temperament, parental speech characteristics, communicative styles, attitudes and knowledge regarding stuttering (e.g., Crowe and Cooper, 1977, Fowlie and Cooper, 1978, Langlois et al., 1986 and Meyers and Freeman, 1985). These facets of PCA may reflect complex associations between the stuttering behaviours and PCA constructs (Seery, Watkins, Mangelsdorf, & Shigeto, 2007). 1.2. Nature and evidence of current research for children who stutter School-aged children (typically aged 6–12 years old) have unique psychosocial, affective and behavioural developmental patterns that are distinct from preschoolers and adolescents. Moving towards adolescence, these children show increasing independence from their parents, and simultaneously, increasing dependence on their peers for social, emotional and academic development (Conture & Guitar, 1993). School-aged children also begin to demonstrate preliminary notions about their sense of self and personal identity, as a reflection of their growing awareness of future socialisation roles. To achieve this successfully, social competence, or skilled communication in social behaviours, plays a substantial role (La Greca & Lopez, 1998). Consequently, the disruptive nature of stuttering may encumber the way some school-aged CWS participate in social discourse. The duration of the experience that the school-aged CWS has had with stuttering is longer than that for a preschooler, but not as chronically advanced as those experiences for adolescents who stutter (Conture and Guitar, 1993 and Schwartz, 1993). The growing demands placed on the school-aged CWS, with expectations for increasingly adult-like social, communicative and academic competence, may exacerbate communication anxiety for some. Therefore, the quality of parent–child relationships might play an important role in providing the child with models of social competence, coping strategies and motivational orientation (Bukatko & Daehler, 2004). Evidence from psychology and psychiatry has suggested that optimal parenting equips children with effective social adjustment and coping resilience, amongst other facets of positive development (Engels et al., 2001, Hoeskma et al., 2004, McGillicuddy-De Lisi and De Lisi, 2007 and Padilla-Walker, 2007). A hallmark longitudinal study found that securely attached children emerged as more enthusiastic and with better problem-solving skills than other children without the same degree of attachment (Ainsworth, 1979). Dismissively attached children were found to be more easily frustrated and less persistent in problem-solving skills. Yet, despite the pervasive influence of parenting, no previous research into attachment construct in relation to school-aged CWS and their parents could be found. A preliminary study by Woods, Shearsby, Onslow, and Burnham (2002) examined attachment and outcomes of the Lidcombe program (an operant-based stuttering therapy program) in preschool children. No systematic relationship was detected and the Lidcombe program was concluded to be safe with no adverse psychological effects. The degree of influence of parent–child relations on school aged children who stutter remains uncertain. In light of these findings, the question remains as to whether distinctive parenting patterns and PCA exist within a population of school-aged CWS compared to their fluent peers; and whether qualitatively, further insights can be gained regarding the attitudes, perceptions and relationships that CWS have with their teachers, peers and parents. Specifically, this study aimed to determine if: (a) particular parenting styles (care and control constructs) differentiate between CWS and their fluent peers; (b) particular parent and peer attachments differentiate CWS and fluent children; (c) self-reported child social behaviour can differentiate CWS from fluent children; (d) parent-reported child social behaviour can differentiate CWS from fluent children; and (e) qualitative interviews can provide further insight with respect to the perceptions, attitudes and social relationships of CWS to parents and peers.