دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 34974
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

مقابله با احساس:پیش بینی مهارت های تنظیم احساسات خاص در پاسخ به استرس در روان پریشی

عنوان انگلیسی
Dealing with feeling: Specific emotion regulation skills predict responses to stress in psychosis
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
34974 2015 7 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Psychiatry Research, Volume 228, Issue 2, 15 August 2015, Pages 216–222

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
روان پریشی - احساسات تنظیم - استرس - قبول آگاهی - پارانویا - سطح رسانایی پوست -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Psychosis; Emotion-regulation; Stress; Acceptance; Awareness; Paranoia; Skin conductance levels
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  مقابله با احساس:پیش بینی مهارت های تنظیم احساسات خاص  در پاسخ به استرس در روان پریشی

چکیده انگلیسی

Elevated negative affect is an established link between minor stressors and psychotic symptoms. Less clear is why people with psychosis fail to regulate distressing emotions effectively. This study tests whether subjective, psychophysiological and symptomatic responses to stress can be predicted by specific emotion regulation (ER) difficulties. Participants with psychotic disorders (n=35) and healthy controls (n=28) were assessed for ER-skills at baseline. They were then exposed to a noise versus no stressor on different days, during which self-reported stress responses, state paranoia and skin conductance levels (SCL) were assessed. Participants with psychosis showed a stronger increase in self-reported stress and SCL in response to the stressor than healthy controls. Stronger increases in self-reported stress were predicted by a reduced ability to be aware of and tolerate distressing emotions, whereas increases in SCL were predicted by a reduced ability to be aware of, tolerate, accept and modify them. Although paranoid symptoms were not significantly affected by the stressors, individual variation in paranoid responses was also predicted by a reduced ability to be aware of and tolerate emotions. Differences in stress responses in the samples were no longer significant after controlling for ER skills. Thus, interventions that improve ER-skills could reduce stress-sensitivity in psychosis.

مقدمه انگلیسی

That environmental stress is implicated in the development of psychotic symptoms can be considered as one of the most convincingly established facts in schizophrenia research (Brown, 2011 and van Os et al., 2010). Furthermore, it is well established that a state of heightened emotionality, often in form of extreme arousal, precedes psychotic symptoms. For example, electrodermal arousal has qualified as a state-sensitive episode indicator (Dawson et al., 1992). Also, several studies using the experience-sampling method to investigate the interplay of stressors, emotions and psychotic symptoms in daily life show that increases in stress are generally associated with an increase in negative affect and psychotic symptoms (e.g. Delespaul et al., 2002 and Myin-Germeys and van Os, 2007), with detailed time-lagged analyses suggesting that negative affect contributes to positive symptoms (Kramer et al., 2014). This is further supported by longitudinal studies showing that baseline mood or mood instability significantly predict later increases in psychotic symptoms over longer periods of time and mediate between early adverse events and later symptoms (Fowler et al., 2012, Freeman et al., 2012 and Marwaha et al., 2014). Experimental studies also corroborate the importance of emotions in psychotic symptom formation. For example, a brief exposure to an urban environment was shown to increase anxiety and paranoid beliefs in patients with delusions (Ellett et al., 2008). Other researchers have found the speech of patients with schizophrenia to become more disordered when negative affect was induced (Cohen and Docherty, 2004). Within groups of psychosis prone participants induction of anxiety caused an increase in paranoid thoughts (Lincoln et al., 2010a) and a bias to perceive neutral faces as angry (Westermann and Lincoln, 2010). Accordingly, newer vulnerability-stress models of psychotic symptoms ascribe emotional distress a central role on the pathway from stress to psychosis (e.g. Garety et al., 2001 and Preti and Celler, 2010). What is less clear is why emotions become so pronounced in people with psychosis and why they translate into psychotic symptoms. Dealing with emotions is generally referred to as emotion regulation (ER; Gross, 2007). During ER, people may increase, maintain, or decrease emotions by means of various strategies, such as reappraisal, acceptance or diversion of attention. These have been conceptualized in a process model that distinguishes between antecedent focused (e.g. attentional deployment, reappraisal) and response focused (e.g. response modulation) strategies (Gross, 1998). Across diagnostic categories the use of strategies aimed at either accepting emotions or at actively modifying them are related to lower symptom levels (Aldao et al., 2010). Findings from the few longitudinal studies available suggest that difficulties in using these skills do not merely accompany but predict subsequent mental health problems (Berking et al., 2011, Ciarrochi and Scott, 2006 and Kassel et al., 2007). Several recent studies point to ER difficulties in people with psychotic disorders: These studies found participants with psychosis to have problems in being aware of emotions (Kimhy et al., 2012, Kimhy et al., 2014 and Lincoln et al., 2015a), which is a precondition for tolerating and accepting them. Compared to healthy controls, people with psychosis also use more dysfunctional and less functional strategies, for example, they tend to suppress rather than accept emotions and are less successful in using cognitive strategies to change emotions in the desired direction (O’Driscoll et al., 2014). Although some studies failed to find ER difficulties related to psychosis (Henry et al., 2008 and Perry et al., 2012), the overall picture is that people with psychosis show increased difficulties in regulating emotions in a functional manner. These difficulties are associated with an array of psychotic symptoms and outcomes. For example, several studies indicated that greater use of suppression went along with an increase in auditory hallucinations (Badcock et al., 2011 and Moritz et al., 2010). Henry et al. (2009) found the tendency to use suppression strategies to correlate with odd beliefs, unusual perceptual experiences and paranoid ideation. Furthermore, the ability to identify and describe emotions has been shown to be positively associated with social functioning in patients with psychosis (Kimhy et al., 2012). Westermann and Lincoln (2011) found general ER deficits to be related to paranoid ideation in a healthy sample. ER deficits even predicted paranoia 1 month later (Westermann et al., 2013). Given these findings, we hypothesize that difficulties in using functional ER skills will help to explain the affective pathway outlined above that links stress to psychotic symptoms. There are two mechanisms by which a lack of functional ER sills could increase the likelihood of symptoms. The first and more straightforward mechanism is that negative affect is not down-regulated and thus becomes so intense that it triggers symptoms. This is the case if extreme arousal results in hallucinations (Dudley et al., 2014) or if extreme anxiety increases the likelihood of threat beliefs (Freeman, 2007). A second mechanism could be that delusions are a dysfunctional way of regulating emotions. This is the case if a delusional belief causes a short-term decrease of a negative affect (Lincoln et al., 2014). Irrespective of which mechanism comes into play, one would expect that more pronounced general ER skills will predict less negative affect following a stressor but also a reduced (or absent) symptomatic response to it. The assumption that ER will predict the stress response is intuitive. It also has important implications for interventions aimed at prevention of symptom formation or relapse. However, so far no study has tested whether and which type of ER skills predict the emotional and symptomatic response to stress in individuals with psychotic disorders. This study thus tests the hypothesis that the subjective, physical and symptomatic response to a stressor in people with psychotic disorders as compared to healthy controls is predicted by their ER skills. We included skin conductance levels as a psychophysiological indicator of arousal, because psychosis has been found to be related to specific difficulties in being aware of and able to describe feelings (alexithymia, Cedro et al., 2001 and Kimhy et al., 2012), which speaks against relying solely on self-report measures. We also aimed to identify the type of ER skills that are best suited to predict the stress response. Based on what has been shown in previous work, we predicted that the ability to be aware of, tolerate or accept, and modify emotions will be associated with less pronounced stress responses. Finally, we tested whether the differences between healthy controls and participants with psychosis in their responses to a stressor would disappear if baseline ER skills were controlled for.