دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 38786
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

اثر حواس پرتی های بصری و شناختی بر روی پیش بینی راننده در یک سناریوی شبیه سازی ماشین

عنوان انگلیسی
The effect of visual and cognitive distraction on driver’s anticipation in a simulated car following scenario
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
38786 2011 12 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Volume 14, Issue 6, November 2011, Pages 555–566

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
پیش بینی - اتومبیل - حواس پرتی - پایان تصادف - آگاهی از وضعیت
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Anticipation; Car following; Distraction; Rear-end crash; Situation awareness
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  اثر حواس پرتی های بصری و شناختی بر روی پیش بینی راننده در یک سناریوی شبیه سازی ماشین

چکیده انگلیسی

Abstract Distraction is a common cause of accidents (e.g. NHTSA, 2009), and different distraction conditions influence the driving behaviour in a specific way. Despite a lot of research in this area, most studies concentrate on perception errors as a result of visual distraction. The effects of different distraction conditions on higher cognitive processes are still unclear. The fact that accidents happen even if the drivers perceive all relevant information implies that cognitive factors contribute to accidents, too. For this reason, this study was conducted to investigate how different distraction conditions influence the anticipation of events in a car–following scenario. Anticipation is required to know what will happen next, and to react adequately to the situation. In a driving simulator, scenarios with different manoeuvres of a preceding car were created to generate various anticipations and therefore a different adaptation of the driving behaviour. Additionally, a cognitive and a visual secondary task were introduced. The question was in which way either a cognitive or visual distraction influences the generation of anticipations and the construction of an appropriate situation model. The results indicate that in the phase when the preceding car showed braking manoeuvres, drivers prolonged their safety distance only when being visually distracted which is probably done to compensate for this visual distraction. This compensation ensued to some extent in the second phase where the preceding cars drove with a constant speed. Additionally, drivers who were visually distracted went somewhat slower when the car in front had braked in phase one. Thus, the drivers seemed to anticipate that the car might brake again and adapted their speed accordingly. This was not found in drivers with cognitive distraction. Thus, cognitive distraction seems to disturb this anticipation. Finally, at an intersection, drivers with visual distraction had a smaller TTC and a higher velocity when the car in front made an unexpected turn. Thus, the impairment of perception due to the visual distraction leads to a slower reaction as compared to a cognitive distraction. Overall, cognitive distraction influences the anticipation of possible future actions of other car drivers negatively while visual distraction deteriorates perception and thus the reaction to critical, sudden events. Thus, different intervention strategies are required to prevent these kinds of accidents.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Introduction A multitude of studies show that distraction while driving is often found (e.g. Stutts and Hunter, 2003, Klauer et al., 2006 and NHTSA, 2009). It is estimated that drivers engage in potentially distracting secondary tasks approximately 30% of the time while their vehicles are in motion (Ranney, 2008). The data from the Fatality Analysis Reporting System (FARS) show that in 16% of all fatal crashes in 2008, driver distraction was involved (NHTSA, 2009). According to data from the General Estimates System (GES), 22% of crashes with injuries were reported to have involved distracted drivers (NHTSA, 2009). In the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study, driver involvement in secondary tasks contributed to over 22% of all crashes and near crashes in the period from 2003 to 2004 (NHTSA, 2009). When looking at accidents, distraction is a primary cause for a special type of accident, the rear-end crash. This is one of the most frequent crash types. Vollrath, Briest, and Drewes (2006) showed, that 26.4% of all severe accidents in Germany in 2002 were rear-end crashes. The German Federal Statistical Office documented that 21.7% of all accidents in the year 2008 were rear-end crashes (German Federal Statistical Office, 2009). According to the NHTSA General Estimates System (GES) rear-end crashes are responsible for approximately 29% of all accidents in 2003 (Najm, Sen, Smith, & Campell, 2003). In order to examine the causes of driver errors leading to a rear-end crash, Muhrer and Vollrath (2009) conducted an in-depth accident analysis using data from Braunschweig 2002 involving expert ratings of accident protocols. They found that decision errors were involved in 86% of the rear-end crashes. Drivers did not select a safe distance towards the preceding car and did not adapt the distance to traffic, road or weather conditions. When a sudden braking manoeuvre of the preceding vehicle occurred in this situation, the driver was not able to react in time. There may be several reasons why drivers did not chose a safe distance which could not be distinguished in the analyses. However, it seems that drivers do not expect that something will happen in this situation and thus do not concentrate as much as necessary on the preceding car. Distraction may be relevant here with regard to two aspects: Drivers may be distracted and that is the cause why they do not notice the braking of the car in front. Additionally, as the situation seems uncritical, drivers may be inattentive and may conduct secondary tasks which then prevent them from adapting their distance and reacting if required. This idea is supported by results from the Indiana Tri-Level study. Treat et al. (1979) analysed 57 rear-end crashes. They found that inattention lead to a delayed recognition of stationary vehicles resulting in rear-end crashes. When rear-end crashes with moving vehicles occurred, a decision error preceded, and drivers did not adapt their behaviour to the car in front. Similarly, Knipling et al. (1992) analysed 74 rear-end crashes and found two common causes for them: inattention, which is more often related to a crash where the vehicle in front is stationary, and following too closely, which is more related to a moving preceding vehicle. Baldock, Long, Lindsay, and Mclean (2005) analysed crashes from the Traffic Accident Reporting System maintained by the South Australian Department for Transport, Energy and Infrastructure. They examined 203,140 accidents that occurred between 1998 and 2002, and 33% of these were classified as rear-end crashes. In their analyses, inattention was the most common cause (73.8%). In 23.5% of the cases following too closely was responsible for the crash. Other studies suggest that distracted drivers are particularly at risk to be involved in rear-end crashes. Green and Shah (2004) postulated that distraction plays a greater role in rear-end crashes than in other crash types. Muhrer (2009) found out that next to the decision error, the perception error because of distraction was the second common reason for the crash. Lee, Llaneras, Klauer, and Sudweeks (2007) analysed 27 rear-end crashes from the database of the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study, and showed that most rear-end crashes occurred in free traffic flow or flow with only few restrictions. They showed that 87% of the drivers were distracted. In 31% of the cases, this distraction had something to do with driving (e.g., checking the mirror, looking out of the window) and led to neglecting the road in front. While these results are based on a very small sample, they fit the overall pattern also found in other studies. Hughes and Cole, 1986 and Stutts et al., 2005 showed that while driving, drivers attend to a range of non-driving stimuli. When distracted by these stimuli, drivers tend to decrease their headway distance (Rosenbloom, 2006), their detection of vehicles decelerating ahead of them is impaired (Lamble, Kauranen, Laakso, & Summala, 1999), and their brake reaction time is significantly increased (Alm and Nilsson, 1995 and Hancock et al., 2003). Distraction may occur at different levels. The most important distinction is that between visual distraction – “eyes off road” – and cognitive distraction – “mind off road”. Both types of distraction interfere in a different way with the driving task. Visual secondary tasks lead to decrements in steering and lane keeping (e.g. Carsten & Brookhuis, 2005). Increased lane keeping variation as a consequence of visual distraction is a typical finding (Engström et al., 2005 and Greenberg et al., 2003). Reduced speed as compensation behaviour (Antin, Dingus, Hulse, & Wierwille, 1990) and impaired event detection performance as a consequence of misperception can also be found (Olsson, 2000). Carsten and Brookhuis (2005) revealed that cognitive secondary tasks impaired car following and reactions to leading cars. Surprisingly, lane keeping improved with cognitive distraction. The analyses of the gaze behaviour showed that drivers focused their attention on the center of the road when cognitive load increased. That may be beneficial for lane keeping. Many studies showed improvement in terms of reduced lane keeping variation in a cognitive distraction condition (Engström et al., 2005, Mattes et al., 2007 and Östlund, 2004). However, there is also an impaired event detection and/or recognition (Recarte & Nunes, 2003). In the Meta analysis of Horrey and Wickens (2004), increased reaction times could also be measured. These studies point out that different distraction conditions influence the driving behaviour in a different way. The question arises how visual and cognitive distraction contributes to the driver errors, leading to rear-end crashes. When we summarise the results of the accident studies, there seem to be two types of errors involved in these rear-end crashes. On the one hand, drivers do not watch the car in front as attentively as they should because they are visually distracted. This may lead to a missing perception of possible manoeuvres of the lead car. According to this interpretation, some studies could show increasing reaction times or missing reactions under visual distraction. On the other hand, drivers do not expect that the car in front will suddenly brake. These missing expectations will have consequences for the driving behaviour and lead to following too closely. The question arises why drivers do not anticipate a required reaction and do not adapt their behaviour to a preceding car. Cognitive distraction may be involved and may lead to inappropriate information processing of the relevant aspects of the situation, and thus no adequate situation model can be generated. This is discussed in the concept of situation awareness. Endsley (1988) described situation awareness as “…the perception of the elements in the environment within a volume of time and space, the comprehension of their meaning, and the projection of their status in the near future…” (p. 97). In this definition situation awareness is composed of three components: First, the perception of elements in a dynamic environment. Second, the integration of the different situation elements into a holistic picture of the situation. Third, the anticipation of future developments of the current situation. Situation awareness is also often mentioned as a factor in the context of a driving task (Baumann and Krems, 2007, Gugerty, 1997, Kass et al., 2007 and Matthews et al., 2001). In the traffic situation, relevant cues like the behaviour of the preceding car (e.g. how will it behave at the next intersection?) and the traffic environment (e.g. traffic density, road conditions …) must be observed by the driver. Based on these cues, expectations are generated about what will happen next and drivers decide how to behave on this basis. This may especially be important in rear-end crashes. If drivers assume that in the near future nothing will happen that requires a reaction they may follow too closely or engage in distracting tasks. Thus, they are not able to react in time and prevent the rear-end crash. Distraction may play a crucial role, as accident analyses have shown that it is one major influencing factor. Visual distraction may influence the first stage of situation awareness, the perception of the elements. Correct expectations cannot be formed because the relevant cues were not perceived. Cognitive distraction may influence later stages of situation awareness like the integration of the cues and the anticipation of the near future. Thus, if cognitive distraction occurs, drivers may perceive the other car but may not be able to anticipate that he or she might brake suddenly and thus will not adapt the headway or prepare for a possible braking. From this point of view one would expect that both visual and cognitive distraction contribute to rear-end accidents. However, the causes of the accident should be different. Furthermore, there is a second assumption how distraction becomes important in rear-end crashes. It might be that only as a result of the missing expectation of a required reaction drivers tend to shift their attention to driving irrelevant tasks and are thus distracted. A driving simulator study was conducted in order to examine these hypotheses. The question was, if the formation of expectations that the preceding car might brake is influenced by visual and cognitive distraction in a different way. When the perception process is the crucial element to build up expectations, then visual distraction may influence the adaptation in a car following scenario and should prevent the driver from adapting to the different traffic situations. When higher cognitive processes are more relevant for the adaptation, then cognitive distraction should impair this adaptation to a greater extent. To examine this, we varied on the one hand the behaviour of the preceding car (braking manoeuvres, signalling turning maneouvres) to generate different expectations about the possible future behaviour of the lead car. On the other hand, visual and cognitive distraction was introduced to examine their influence on the generation of these expectations. Finally, it was examined if drivers tend to look more often to the secondary task, when the current situation does not lead to the expectation that a manoeuvre will be required.