دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 30408
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

آیا مزاحمت سایبری با زورگویی در مدرسه هنگام گرفتن نحوه مشارکت همپوشانی دارد؟

عنوان انگلیسی
Does cyberbullying overlap with school bullying when taking modality of involvement into account?
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
30408 2015 9 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Computers in Human Behavior, Volume 43, February 2015, Pages 49–57

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
- مزاحمت سایبری - قلدری مدرسه - رسانه ها - رفتارهای برونی - مشکلات درونی
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Cyberbullying,School bullying,Media,Externalizing behaviors,Internalizing problems
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  آیا مزاحمت سایبری با زورگویی در مدرسه هنگام گرفتن نحوه مشارکت همپوشانی دارد؟

چکیده انگلیسی

Education professionals and researchers are concerned by school bullying and cyberbullying because of its repercussions on students’ health and the school climate. However, only a few studies investigating the impact of school versus cyberbullying have systematically explored whether student victims and perpetrators are involved in school bullying only, cyberbullying only, or both. The aim of the present study was thus to examine the possible overlap, as well as the similarities and/or differences, between these two forms of bullying when taking modality of involvement into account. Individual interviews were conducted with 1422 junior high- and high-school students (girls = 43%, boys = 57%, mean age = 14.3 ± 2.7 years). Results showed that cyberbullying and school bullying overlapped very little. The majority of students involved in cyberbullying were not simultaneously involved in school bullying. Moreover, results indicated that psychosocial problems (psychological distress, social disintegration, general aggression) varied according to the form of bullying. Victims of school bullying had greater internalizing problems than cybervictims, while school bullies were more aggressive than cyberbullies. Given the sizable proportion of adolescents involved in bullying (school and cyber) and its significant relationship with mental health, the issue warrants serious attention from school and public health authorities.

مقدمه انگلیسی

1.1. School bullying and cyberbullying Bullying is a pervasive form of aggressive behavior that has been studied in many countries (Craig et al., 2009 and Menesini et al., 2012) and many different research areas, including psychology, medicine and biology, etc. It is devastating for the school climate and more especially for students’ wellbeing, leading it to be classified as a major public-health problem in schools (Steffgen et al., 2013 and Turner et al., 2013). Bullying is an intentional strategy engaged in by one or more student(s), who set up an asymmetrical relationship with a classmate based on physical or psychological power. Olweus (1993) identified three criteria to define bullying: 1) it is an aggressive behavior that is intentional; 2) it is repetitive; and 3) it is an interpersonal relation characterized by a systematic imbalance of power and domination. Four profiles can be identified in this kind of relationship: neutral, victim, bully, and bully-victim, this last profile referring to students who are the victims of bullying and who bully classmates other than their own aggressors. In schools, bullying can manifest itself either in direct behaviors, be they physical (slapping, pushing, etc.) or verbal (insults; etc.), or in indirect attacks (spreading rumors about a student and/or organizing his/her social exclusion) (Stassen-Berger, 2007). Over the past few years, a new form of bullying has emerged and caught the attention of researchers and education professionals. The huge advances in digital technology have given young people new means of communicating, but also brought some deleterious social interactions such as cyberbullying (Kowalski & Limber, 2007). Most definitions of cyberbullying come from definitions of school bullying. Thus, this conduct is often described as an intentional aggressive behavior that takes place via new technologies, during which groups or individuals hurt classmates who cannot easily defend themselves (Kowalski and Limber, 2007, Law et al., 2010 and Slonje et al., 2013). Cyberbullying events can occur via cellphones or computers, by means of text messages, e-mails, online social networks (e.g., Facebook®, Twitter®), chatrooms or blogs (Kowalski & Limber, 2007). Like the bullying that occurs in school, the following four profiles have been identified: cyberneutral, cyberbully, cybervictim and cyberbully-victim. Although there are many cyberbullying strategies around, Cowie (2013) has identified some of the most frequently occurring ones. Denigration, for instance, consists in posting false information, gossip or rumors about a classmate on a blog or an online social network in order to damage his/her reputation or friendships. Entering the mailbox or the personal online space of a classmate and then usurping his/her identity to send or post material to get that person into trouble or damage his/her reputation or friendships is another strategy that is used. Repeatedly sending mean, insulting or threatening messages is also an example of cyberbullying. Contrary to the consensus on the three criteria for defining school bullying, there is no single clear and consistent definition of cyberbullying (Patchin & Hinduja, 2013). Moreover, many different words are used in the literature to depict these online practices besides the term cyberbullying, including online harassment ( Wolak, Mitchell, & Finkelhor, 2007), electronic bullying ( Kowalski and Limber, 2007 and Raskauskas and Stoltz, 2007), Internet harassment ( Ybarra, Espelage, et al., 2007 and Ybarra et al., 2006) and e-bullying ( Lam & Li, 2013). However, it should be noted that some of the researchers who initially used these other notions, now employ the word cyberbullying (e.g., Kowalski et al., 2014 and Ybarra et al., 2012). The heterogeneity of the devices considered in studies of cyberbullying is another example of the divergence in definitions. Some researchers have investigated behaviors via cellphones and/or computers (Kowalski and Limber, 2007, Ortega et al., 2009 and Raskauskas and Stoltz, 2007), others have only taken one of these devices into account (Aricak et al., 2008 and Wolak et al., 2007). These observations probably go some way to explaining the disparity in the figures for cyberbullying prevalence. Estimated rates of cyberbullying vary from 11% to more than 50% in studies considering cybervictimization, cyberperpetration, and both (Kowalski et al., 2014). Despite the disparities in estimated cyberbullying prevalence around the world, one finding that appears to be common and convergent is that involvement in bullying in cyberspace is associated with psychosocial problems, problematic Internet use and poor school performances (Gámez-Guadix et al., 2013 and Kowalski and Limber, 2013). In France, very few data are available concerning the number of students involved in cyberbullying and the attendant psychosocial difficulties. Even so, this form of aggressive behavior may well affect many French adolescents, as in France, 95% of 9- to 16-year-olds use the Internet at home (Livingstone, Haddon, Görzig, & Ólafsson, 2011), thus increasing the risk of being involved in cyberattacks (Kwan & Skoric, 2013). 1.2. Divergent considerations Currently, one of the main questions being explored in the scientific literature concerns the degree of overlap between cyberbullying and school bullying: do they constitute the same kind of aggressive behavior, with cyberbullying being a modern and electronic form of school bullying? Or are they two forms of aggressive behavior that need to be differentiated? Studies have yielded very divergent results. Some of them suggest that cyberbullying is closely linked to school bullying, possibly constituting an extension of it, whereas other studies indicate that cyberbullying does not mirror school bullying. The arguments evoked in these studies cite observed prevalence, as well as the psychosocial characteristics associated with the various profiles in these two forms of bullying. Studies suggesting that school bullying and cyberbullying considerably overlap (Juvonen and Gross, 2008, Raskauskas and Stoltz, 2007, Smith et al., 2008 and Wang et al., 2009) have shown that victims in schools tend also to be victims in cyberspace, and cyberbullies are often students who perpetrate bullying at school. For example, in the study by Raskauskas and Stoltz (2007), 94% of cyberbullies were also school bullies, and 85% of cybervictims had a victim profile at school. One year later, Juvonen and Gross (2008) published results revealing similar tendencies: among the 1454 adolescents they sampled, 85% of those involved in cyberbullying were also involved in school bullying. These data led the authors of these articles to hypothesize that cyberbullying is the cyberspace extension of school bullying. In line with this hypothesis, other surveys have revealed that students who are victims of school bullying also engage in cyberbullying as bully, often attacking their school aggressors in cyberspace (König, Gollwitzer, & Steffgen, 2010). The fact that more than half of all cyberbullies or cyberbullies/victims are the target of bullying in schools further supports the idea of extension (Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004). Other arguments help to sustain the overlap hypotheses. Thus, some studies have shown that students matching the different school and cyberbullying profiles share similar psychosocial difficulties. As an illustration, it appear that being a cybervictim and being a victim of school bullying are both significant predictors of social anxiety (Juvonen & Gross, 2008). Moreover, both forms of bullying lead to the same distress for victims (Smith et al., 2008) and share interrelated predictors (Casas, Del Rey, & Ortega-Ruiz, 2013). However, all too few studies have carefully considered the modalities of bullying involvement, that is, whether students engage in school bullying only, cyberbullying only, or both (Kowalski et al., 2014 and Olweus, 2012). Research in this area of investigation needs to control for the fact that a student involved in cyberbullying could also be engaged in school bullying, but this precaution is rarely taken. As a consequence, in many studies that fail to control for involvement in both forms of bullying, the psychosocial problems found to be associated with cyberbullying could, in fact, be mainly linked to school bullying (or vice versa). As stated by Olweus (2012), there is a need to find out the effects of cyberbullying independently of the possible effects of school bullying. However, this issue has not received “much systematic and useful research attention so far” (Olweus, 2012). Whereas some studies have shown a close relationship between school bullying and cyberbullying, others led to differentiate these two forms of aggressive behavior. Contradicting the prevalence estimates mentioned above, Ybarra, Diener-West, and Leaf (2007) demonstrated that most victims of cyberbullying are not victims at school. Similarly, Kowalski and Limber (2013) found that most students involved in school bullying (77% of school victims, 74% of school bullies and 52% of school bully-victims) are not concerned by cyberbullying at all. Moreover, if cyberbullying were indeed an extension of school bullying, then homeschooled young people would be protected from cyberbullying. However, cybervictimization rates do not differ significantly between homeschooling and public/private schooling (Ybarra, Diener-West, et al., 2007 and Ybarra, Espelage, et al., 2007). This second consideration is also supported by research on the difficulties associated with school bullying and cyberbullying. Thus, Wang, Nansel, and Iannotti (2011) found a differential association of depression with each of these aggressive behaviors: in school bullying, both victims and bully-victims had higher levels of depression than bullies, whereas in cyberbullying, only cybervictims exhibited higher levels of depression, and to a far greater degree. Moreover, in a study by Ortega et al. (2009), victims were revealed to be less emotionally affected in cases of cyberbullying than in cases of school bullying. By contrast, Hay, Meldrum, and Mann (2010) described higher levels of psychosocial problems in cyberbullying than in school bullying. Again, it should be noted that most of the studies yielding these kinds of results failed to control for the co-occurrence of bullying and cyberbullying. 1.3. Singularity of cyberbullying Above and beyond the above-mentioned divergences regarding prevalence and psychosocial problems, cyberbullying can be distinguished from school bullying on many other dimensions. Although many cyberattacks are similar to those perpetrated in schools (threats, insults, etc.) some have no equivalent in school bullying. This is the case for the creation of a virtual group targeting a schoolmate or the hacking of someone’s personal space. Moreover, cyberbullying is not limited in either time or place (Kowalski and Limber, 2007 and Raskauskas and Stoltz, 2007). While school bullying is often restricted to the time when the victim is present in school, aggression via new technologies can occur at any time of the day or night, and can pursue students into their homes, and even into their bedrooms. In schools, bullies can often be identified and, to a certain extent, avoided. In cyberspace, attacks can be rendered anonymous through the use of pseudonyms, causing considerable distress for victims (Kowalski and Limber, 2007, Li, 2007, Mishna et al., 2009 and Smith et al., 2008; Ybarra, Diener-West, et al., 2007 and Ybarra, Espelage, et al., 2007). Instant and massive dissemination is another particular feature of cyberbullying. Most episodes of school bullying occur in front of only a few schoolmates (Olweus & Limber, 2010), whereas in cyberspace, information and files can be spread quickly and remain available for a long time, thereby increasing the number of potential bystanders (Kowalski et al., 2014 and Li, 2007). Lastly, unlike face-to-face relationships, the use of media such as cellphones or computers lacks nonverbal communication (Kowalski et al., 2014). Cyberbullies cannot visualize the emotional state and pain they inflict on their victims. Many authors have suggested that the lack of direct feedback, combined with the possibility of retaining one’s anonymity, prompts individuals to engage in even more hostile and aggressive behaviors (Postmes, Spears, & Lea, 1998). One of the variants of Milgram’s famous experiment (1974) incidentally corroborates this notion. He showed that when participants could not see or hear the suffering simulated by the actor to whom they had to administer electric shocks, these mock electric shocks more often reached a lethal intensity, compared with situations in which the participants received emotional feedback. Cyberspace therefore fosters disinhibition, as neatly illustrated in a study by Aricak et al. (2008), who found that 59% of students in their sample admitted to saying things online that they would not say face to face. 1.4. Objective and hypotheses Although several studies have already explored the overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying, the results are often divergent and need to be confirmed by further quantitative and qualitative analyses. Moreover, as mentioned earlier, many studies have investigated the problems associated with school bullying and cyberbullying without controlling for the effect of being involved in both forms of bullying. The main aim of the present study was thus to measure the degree of overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying regarding prevalence, and identify the psychosocial problems associated with each of these aggressive behaviors. To this end, we took the precaution of controlling for simultaneous involvement in school and cyberbullying, and carried out quantitative and more qualitative analyses. In the light of the literature mentioned in Section 1.3., showing the singularity of cyberbullying at several different levels, we hypothesized that it constitutes a different form of aggressive behavior that needs to be distinguished from school bullying. More precisely, we tested the following hypotheses: H1. The majority of students involved in cyberbullying are not simultaneously involved in school bullying; H2. The psychosocial characteristics (internalizing problems and externalizing behaviors) of students involved in cyberbullying differ from those of students involved in school bullying.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

The results of this study lead to the conclusion that cyberbullying is a form of aggressive behavior that is quite distinct from school bullying. These two forms were differentiated both from the perspective of prevalence and from the perspective of the psychosocial problems associated with the different profiles. In other words, not only were different students involved in each of these forms of bullying, but they also had distinct characteristics. These conclusions could contribute to the design of preventive interventions in schools. Given the difference between school and cyberbullying, it is difficult to conceive that actions intended to reduce school bullying could be as efficient in preventing cyberbullying. Because of the undeniable presence of cyberbullying and the number of young people concerned, students, together with their parents and teachers, should be regularly informed of the existence of this practice and any changes it undergoes. Having the means to protect oneself and to deal with school or cyberbullying are other aspects that need to be tackled, given the psychosocial problems that can be generated. Despite the differences between these two kinds of bullying, they need to be combatted simultaneously, through a combination of targeted actions.