دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 32458
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

ارواح پایین پاداش را تسخیر می کنند: کاهش حساسیت به پاداش و آسیب پذیری برای بی قراری

عنوان انگلیسی
Low Spirits Keep Rewards Subdued: Decreases in Sensitivity to Reward and Vulnerability to Dysphoria
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
32458 2013 13 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Behavior Therapy, Volume 44, Issue 1, March 2013, Pages 62–74

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
/ افسردگی - خلق و خوی - مجازات - جایزه -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
BIS/BAS; depression; mood; punishment; reward
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  ارواح پایین پاداش را تسخیر می کنند: کاهش حساسیت به پاداش و آسیب پذیری برای بی قراری

چکیده انگلیسی

Previous theories and research show clear divergences on the roles of the behavioral activation system (BAS) and the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) in depression. Across four studies, we examined the effects of a sad mood on the motivational pattern of sensitivity to reward and punishment. Psychological variables associated with such changes and implications for vulnerability to depression were also explored. For this purpose, we designed a state version of the extensively used BIS/BAS Scales (Carver & White, 1994). Using samples of undergraduate students, we found that both a natural (Study 1) and a laboratory-induced sad mood (Studies 2 and 3) generated a marked decrease in sensitivity to reward but did not alter sensitivity to punishment. Study 3a showed that participants’ anxious attachment predicted larger decreases in sensitivity to reward after a sad mood induction. Study 3b extended these results by showing that sensitivity to reward, when assessed after the negative mood induction, predicted increases in dysphoria 7 weeks later. Implications of the results for research on vulnerability to depression are discussed.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Many authors (e.g., Fowles, 1993, Gray, 1987 and Lang, 1995) have proposed the existence of two independent motivational systems for the regulation of behavior. Research on these motivational systems has mainly been conducted within the framework of Gray's (1987) reinforcement sensitivity theory. Gray's original model suggested that there are two neurobiologically independent systems of appetitive and aversive motivation. The behavioral activation system (BAS) motivates behavior in response to cues for reward and absence of punishment, and it is also responsible for escape behaviors. The behavioral inhibition system (BIS), in contrast, motivates behavior in response to cues for punishment and absence of reward, controlling for the reaction to novel stimuli. Activation of the BAS is associated with behaviors of approach toward desirable outcomes, feelings of euphoria, and reward seeking. Activation of the BIS is associated with avoidance behaviors, feelings of anxiety in the presence of signs of punishment, and feelings of frustration in the absence of reward (Gray, 1987).1 Although the BIS/BAS proposal has been elaborated and investigated in a number of different respects (e.g., Gray & McNaughton, 2000), its implications for the field of psychopathology are particularly important (see Bijttebier, Beck, Claes, & Vandereycken, 2010, for a review). Gray's theory has had the most significant impact on the study of affective disorders. Regarding depression, Gray suggested that individuals exhibiting greater sensitivity to punishment would be more vulnerable to anxious–depressive disorders, whereas greater sensitivity to reward would be related to impulsivity disorders (Gray, 1987). Later, Gray (1991) also hypothesized that low sensitivity to reward might also be related to anhedonic depression. Similarly, the tripartite model of anxiety and depression proposed that low sensitivity to reward and low positive affect are specific to depression (L. A. Clark & Watson, 1991) but also noted that the BIS is a negative motivational-affective system that is important both in depression and anxiety (L. A. Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994; see also Zinbarg & Yoon, 2008). Thus, different theories suggest that depression is associated with lower sensitivity to reward and higher sensitivity to punishment. Various studies have confirmed that depressed individuals present significantly lower levels of sensitivity to reward (Kasch et al., 2002 and Pinto-Meza et al., 2006) and higher levels of sensitivity to punishment (Johnson et al., 2003 and Pinto-Meza et al., 2006). However, other theories have argued that only sensitivity to reward is relevant to understanding depression (Depue and Iacono, 1989 and Fowles, 1993) and many research findings support this proposal. For example, although several studies have shown that clinically depressed individuals present reduced reactivity to positive stimuli in general (e.g., Rottenberg, Kasch, Gross, & Gotlib, 2002; see Bylsma, Morris, & Rottenberg, 2008, for a review) and to rewards in particular (Henriques and Davidson, 2000, Henriques et al., 1994 and McFarland and Klein, 2008), they failed to find elevated reactivity or sensitivity to negative stimuli or punishments. The same pattern of results has been observed when analyzing life goals of depressed adolescents. Dickson and MacLeod (2004) found that depression was associated with an approach-motivational deficit but not with augmented avoidance motivation. In longitudinal studies, only sensitivity to reward and reactivity to positive stimuli, not the BIS or reactivity to negative stimuli, have been able to predict better recovery of depressed patients (Kasch et al., 2002, McFarland et al., 2006 and Rottenberg et al., 2002). On the other hand, the only study to our knowledge that examined the ability of sensitivity to reward or anhedonia to predict increases in depressive symptoms over time failed to find significant results (D. C. Clark, Salazar-Gruesco, Grabler, & Fawcett, 1984). Thus, sensitivity to punishment has not been found to be relevant for depression in laboratory or longitudinal studies. Laboratory studies have found a BAS deficit in depressed individuals, but longitudinal studies have yielded divergent results depending on the sample used: BAS scores predicted changes in depressive symptoms in clinical samples, but no significant results were found when a nondepressed sample was used. What could be the reason for these contradictory findings? Motivational vulnerability may remain latent when individuals are in a euthymic mood state and be activated by negative life events. Consequently, it is possible that only sensitivity to reward assessed after the mood induction, but not before, predicts increases in depressive symptoms. Indeed, some prior work supports this mood-dependent hypothesis. Research on vulnerability to depression has found that a sad mood can activate vulnerability processes that remain concealed during a euthymic mood. For example, some studies have found that negative cognitive biases can predict increases of depressive symptoms over time when assessed after a sad mood induction but not when assessed during a euthymic mood (e.g., Beevers & Carver, 2003). Thus, exploring changes in sensitivity to punishment and reward after a sad mood may help to explain the role of such variables in the development of depression. In summary, some researchers have suggested that depression is associated with a reduced sensitivity to reward and an increased sensitivity to punishment. However, others have hypothesized that only sensitivity to reward is associated with depression. Overall, results from longitudinal studies and from studies using methods other than self-report suggest that only sensitivity to reward is associated with depression. However, it is not entirely clear whether sensitivity to reward predicts an increase in depressive symptoms over time when a nondepressed sample is used. Exploring the effects of the onset of a depressed mood on the motivational systems could shed light on these contradictory results. Surprisingly, the causal relationship between mood and changes in sensitivity to reward and punishment has not been explored yet. Perhaps the most important explanation for this absence of studies is the lack of self-report measures that assess state sensitivity to punishment and reward. Therefore, we developed a state measure of sensitivity to reward and punishment based on the BIS/BAS Scales (Carver & White, 1994), the characteristics of which will be detailed below. Some indirect evidence indicates that changes in sensitivity to punishment or reward after the onset of a sad mood are plausible. For instance, it has been found that hedonic capacity, which is theoretically related to the BAS, might diminish after a negative mood (Carson & Adams, 1980). More recently, another line of research has demonstrated that stress induction may reduce reward responsiveness (Bogdan & Pizzagalli, 2006). In the present study, we explored whether the onset of a sad mood would produce changes in sensitivity to reward, in sensitivity to punishment, or both in order to understand the inconsistencies found across theoretical and empirical evidence. According to some of the previous research, but contrary to Gray's (1987) theoretical model, we expected that only the BAS would be affected by the onset of a sad mood. Furthermore, we also aimed to evaluate whether sensitivity to reward, sensitivity to punishment, or both are related to vulnerability to depression. To this end, we evaluated whether changes in sensitivity to reward and punishment after the onset of a sad mood were related to other variables typically associated with vulnerability to depression. Finally, we evaluated whether BIS/BAS scores assessed both before and after the onset of a negative mood were able to predict dysphoria over time. We expected that only BIS/BAS scores assessed after the mood induction would predict increases in dysphoria. In summary, the present investigation included a naturalistic study, two laboratory studies, and a longitudinal study to better explore the role of sensitivity to reward and punishment in vulnerability to depression. Study 1 In Study 1, we assessed state BIS/BAS scores before and after the natural emergence of a sad mood. We used a diary study (as described by Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003) to test the hypothesis that decreases in mood would be followed by decreases in sensitivity to reward but not by increases in sensitivity to punishment. Specifically, we used an event-based design (Bolger et al., 2003). This methodology allowed us to study the target response (i.e., a sad mood) in its natural and spontaneous context. In this study, the onset of a sad mood was the event that signaled participants to complete the questionnaires. Participants first completed a set of questionnaires in the laboratory and then completed another set immediately after the natural emergence of a sad mood.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

Cognitive, personality, and interpersonal theories have sought to explain vulnerability to depression, but the role of motivational factors, especially in relation to other well-validated theories, has been largely neglected. We hope this research encourages future investigations on whether and how motivation intertwines with other vulnerability processes increasing the probability of suffering clinical depressive episodes.