حافظه کاذب، فاقد شرح ادراکی: شواهدی از اتمام کلام بنیادی ضمنی و آزمون شناسایی ادراکی
کد مقاله | سال انتشار | تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی |
---|---|---|
32870 | 2005 | 13 صفحه PDF |
Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)
Journal : Journal of Memory and Language, Volume 52, Issue 3, April 2005, Pages 309–321
چکیده انگلیسی
We used implicit measures of memory to ascertain whether false memories for critical nonpresented items in the DRM paradigm (Deese, 1959 and Roediger and McDermott, 1995) contain structural and perceptual detail. In Experiment 1, we manipulated presentation modality in a visual word-stem-completion task. Critical item priming was significant and unaffected by modality. In contrast, priming of critical items was absent in a perceptual identification test when only DRM list items were studied (Experiment 2A), whereas priming was found when critical items were studied (Experiment 2B). Standard modality effects were present for list items in each experiment and for critical items in Experiment 2B. We conclude that: (a) false memories do not inherently contain structural and perceptual information and (b) past reports of critical item priming relied on implicit tests more prone to conceptual activation.
مقدمه انگلیسی
The Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm has been used extensively to investigate the creation and retrieval of false memories (Deese, 1959 and Roediger and McDermott, 1995). In the standard procedure, people learn lists of items (e.g., table, desk, swivel, etc.) associatively related to a critical, nonpresented theme word (e.g., chair; henceforth referred to as the critical item). These encoding conditions produce reliable, and very often, high levels of false memory for the critical item (e.g., Hicks and Marsh, 2001, Payne et al., 1996, Read, 1996 and Roediger and McDermott, 1995). One reason for the great amount of interest in this paradigm is that people often report false memories with high confidence and/or vividness (e.g., Norman and Schacter, 1997 and Roediger and McDermott, 1995). People are also willing to make attributions concerning the source or context in which the critical item was ostensibly encountered (e.g., Hicks and Hancock, 2002, Hicks and Marsh, 1999, Hicks and Marsh, 2001, Lampinen et al., 1999, Marsh and Hicks, 2000 and Payne et al., 1996). The work on false recognition and source attributions suggests that people may be retrieving vivid perceptual details associated with critical items (see Hicks & Hancock, 2002; for one possible explanation). As compared to true memories, however, false memories do not contain perceptual attributes to the same extent. For example, Norman and Schacter (1997) reported that perceptual characteristics, such as how words sounded, were more available for studied items as compared to critical items (see also Mather, Henkel, & Johnson, 1997; for comparable data). One conclusion drawn from these reports is that retrieval of specific perceptual attributes does not accompany false memory retrieval, or at least that such attributes are not as vivid or plentiful in false memories as compared to true memories. Rather, false memories appear to be associated primarily with conceptual detail, such as associations formed during encoding. However, a line of investigation into the indirect retrieval of false memories suggests that perceptual detail is associated with the activation of critical items in the DRM paradigm. A handful of studies have shown priming for nonstudied critical items on perceptual implicit memory tests, such as word-stem completion (McDermott, 1997, McKone and Murphy, 2000 and Smith et al., 2002), word-fragment completion (McDermott, 1997), and anagram solution (Lövdén & Johansson, 2003). Two other studies have also shown priming of critical items in a lexical decision task (Hancock et al., 2003 and Whittlesea, 2002), although in two others using lexical decision critical items showed no evidence of priming (McKone, 2004 and Zeelenberg and Pecher, 2002). Nor was priming found in a word naming task (Whittlesea, 2002). Notwithstanding these latter null results, the collective findings are remarkable in that they demonstrate visual perceptual priming for concepts not visually perceived. Demonstrations of perceptual critical item priming are significant because they challenge the view that critical items are primarily the result of conceptual activation in a semantic network (e.g., Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001). To reconcile this paradox, McKone and Murphy (2000) proposed that perceptual critical item priming in the absence of perceptual encoding is caused by modality-specific conceptual activation. Specifically, they suggested that studying DRM lists promotes implicit associative responses (IARs) of critical items (e.g., Underwood, 1965) and that these IARs also involve the activation of surface form consistent with the source of activation, such as list words’ modality of presentation. In other words, the IAR is akin to a visual or auditory image. Although the initial activation is conceptual (i.e., associative) in nature, that activation takes some modality-specific form. McKone and Murphy used the IAR/imagery hypothesis to explain why visual word-stem completion produced significant critical item priming when study lists were presented visually, whereas visual word-stem completion did not show significant critical item priming when study lists were presented aurally in a separate experiment. In other words, they not only found a modality effect for critical items, but when study was auditory but testing was visual, the level of priming (about .06) was not statistically significant. Lövdén and Johansson (2003) argued that studying DRM list items promotes a covert verbal recoding of the critical item to explain why they found priming in an anagram solution task. We consider their recoding hypothesis to be similar in spirit to McKone and Murphy’s, and therefore we classify it as a variant of the IAR/imagery hypothesis. One obvious concern about the aforementioned priming demonstrations is that the implicit tests used may be prone to explicit contamination (e.g., Bowers and Schacter, 1990 and Schacter et al., 1989). In other words, people may consciously use recently studied information to perform implicit memory tests. Two of the studies showing critical item priming have employed procedures to either reduce or assess the impact of explicit memory (McKone and Murphy, 2000 and Smith et al., 2002). The current study addresses the issue of explicit contamination in a number of ways. First, the implicit memory tests include predominantly words not seen in any previous phase of the experiment. Second, participants in each experiment completed a post-experiment questionnaire assessing retrieval strategies for the implicit task, and all priming analyses were evaluated both with and without those participants who report attempting to remember words from an earlier phase of the experiment. Third, the first experiment includes two conditions in which participants encode the study lists incidentally; thus, participants in these conditions hear no mention of a memory test either at encoding or retrieval. Fourth, as discussed later in more detail, the perceptual identification task we used in the remaining experiments is less prone to explicit contamination by its very nature. The primary goal of this study is to thoroughly test the IAR/imagery account of perceptual critical item priming. Following McKone and Murphy (2000), we manipulated presentation modality as one way to test this hypothesis. The modality specificity of presented item priming in perceptual implicit memory tests is a standard finding, demonstrating that the perceptual processes used to encode these stimuli play a large role in their subsequent priming (e.g., Jacoby & Dallas, 1981). According to the IAR/imagery hypothesis, critical item priming is based on a similar perceptual encoding (i.e., imagining perceptual details instead of directly perceiving them). Thus, this hypothesis predicts that critical item priming should show the same effects of modality as presented item priming. We also explore the IAR/imagery hypothesis by evaluating critical item priming on two different types of perceptual implicit memory tests. In Experiment 1, we used a stem-completion test. Other researchers have demonstrated critical item priming on this type of test, and we expected to replicate this result. In Experiment 2, we tested for implicit memory using perceptual identification, a type of test in which participants are presented with a stimulus that is difficult to perceive and simply asked to identify it. Perceptual identification should be particularly informative in evaluating the IAR/imagery hypothesis, because previous studies suggest that the demands of perceptual identification are more perceptually restrictive than the demands of stem completion (e.g., Burgund and Marsolek, 1997, Jacoby and Dallas, 1981 and Marsolek and Andresen, in press). By contrast, word-stem completion is affected by a variety of representations, including visual (Graf et al., 1985 and Marsolek et al., 1992), conceptual and semantic (e.g., Bassili et al., 1989 and Keane et al., 1991), and phonological (e.g., Badgaiyan et al., 1999 and Rueckl and Mathew, 1999). We expect to find significant priming of list items in perceptual identification, because these items will be associated with perceptual information. The IAR/imagery hypothesis attributes critical item priming to perceptual information, so this account also predicts significant critical item priming in the perceptual identification test. In contrast, if critical item priming is absent on perceptual identification tests, this would suggest that perceptual priming is not available for critical items in this relatively more restrictive task (e.g., Marsolek & Andresen, in press). Experiment 1 Our first experiment was a partial replication of previous reports using a visual word-stem-completion task. McKone and Murphy (2000) used auditory and visual presentation in separate experiments. We manipulated within-subjects whether the DRM lists were auditory or visual. Therefore, the present study marks the first direct manipulation of presentation modality in the context of a single experiment in an investigation of critical item priming. If the IAR/imagery hypothesis is correct, we should find more priming for critical items when their corresponding lists themes were studied visually.