دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 32910
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

تفاوت های فردی در حافظه کاذب از اطلاعات غلط: ویژگی های شخصیتی و تعامل آنها با توانایی های شناختی

عنوان انگلیسی
Individual differences in false memory from misinformation: Personality characteristics and their interactions with cognitive abilities
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
32910 2010 6 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Personality and Individual Differences, Volume 48, Issue 8, June 2010, Pages 889–894

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
حافظه کاذب - ویژگی های شخصیتی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
False memory; Personality characteristics
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  تفاوت های فردی در حافظه کاذب از اطلاعات غلط: ویژگی های شخصیتی و تعامل آنها با توانایی های شناختی

چکیده انگلیسی

This research investigated the associations among personality characteristics, cognitive abilities, and false memory induced by misinformation. Chinese college students (N = 436) participated in a misinformation study and received a battery of cognitive tasks and personality measures. Results showed that false memory was positively related to persistence, self-directedness, and active coping, but negatively related to depression, fear of negative evaluation, novelty seeking, negative coping, and cognitive abilities. Importantly, significant interaction effects were found between personality factors and cognitive abilities. Individuals with particular combinations of personality characteristics and cognitive abilities (i.e., low fear of negative evaluation, low harm avoidance, high cooperativeness, high reward dependence, and high self-directedness in combination with relatively low cognitive abilities) were more vulnerable to the misinformation effect.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Past research on false memories has primarily focused on the experimental conditions that can create different amounts and types of false memories (Loftus, 2005). Recently, this area of research has experienced a growing interest in individual differences in false memory (Eisen, Winograd, & Qin, 2002). Although not always consistent, accumulating evidence shows that individuals with certain personality and cognitive characteristics show greater propensity for experiencing false memories. In our recent study of cognitive factors in false memory (Zhu et al., submitted for publication), for example, we found that cognitive abilities were negatively associated with greater likelihood of having false memory. In this study, we extended our previous research to include personality factors in false memory. The present study had both hypothesis-testing and exploratory components. A number of studies have linked personality and other individual characteristics to false memories. Few of them, however, have used the misinformation paradigm. Therefore, the first aim of the current study was to extend previous findings to misinformation false memory. We used a large sample and an extensive battery of measures of personality and related constructs. Given our previous findings on the importance of cognitive factors in misinformation false memories, this study’s second aim was to explore whether personality and cognitive abilities had interactive effects on misinformation false memory. In the following paragraphs, we first review previous literature on the personality factors in false memory that are most relevant to the current study, and then discuss the rationale for our exploratory examination of potential interactions between personality and cognitive abilities on false memory. 1.1. Personality factors and false memory 1.1.1. Dimensional personality traits Several studies have correlated measures of dimensional personality traits with individual differences in false memory. Porter, Birt, Yuille, and Lehman (2000) found that subjects scoring low on extraversion were more susceptible to creating false childhood memories. Liebman et al. (2002) found that false memory from misinformation (labeled as suggestibility in that study) was positively associated with three subscales of the NEO Personality Inventory (values of the Openness dimension, and modesty and altruism of the Agreeableness dimension). Salthouse and Siedlecki (2007), however, did not find significant correlations between personality traits and false memory of words as measured by the Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) method (i.e., lists of semantically related words can lead to false memories of seeing the critical “lure” words that were not presented, Roediger and McDermott (1995)). 1.1.2. Dissociation Dissociative experience, or the tendency to have disturbances of normal integration of awareness, thought, memory and personality (Carlson & Putnam, 1993), has weak to moderate associations with several kinds of false memories (Eisen & Lynn, 2001), including false childhood memories (Qin, Ogle, & Goodman, 2008), errors induced by misleading questions (Merckelbach, Muris, Rassin, & Horselenberg, 2000), and DRM false memory (Winograd, Peluso, & Glover, 1998). Some studies have not replicated this finding (e.g., Wilkinson & Hyman, 1998). 1.1.3. Depression Most of the significant findings regarding the positive relation between depression and false memory came from special populations, such as subjects with PTSD or self-reported alien abduction (Clancy et al., 2002 and Zoellner et al., 2000). Using a non-clinical sample, Salthouse and Siedlecki (2007), however, did not find a significant relation between DRM false memory and depression. Storbeck and Clore (2005) further found that negative mood was related to less DRM false memory. 1.1.4. Anxiety In a study of patients with PTSD, Zoellner et al. (2000) found that DRM false recall was positively related to anxiety. Similarly, Gudjonsson (1988) found that social-evaluative anxiety (measured by the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale) was positively associated with interrogative suggestibility (a form of susceptibility to false memory as induced by misleading questions). In contrast, Roberts (2002) found that when under stress (i.e., right before the final examinations), students with low anxiety were more likely to have false memory of words as pictures. 1.1.5. Coping strategies Gudjonsson (2003) emphasized the importance of coping strategies in the suggestion process. Gudjonsson (1988) found that subjects who used avoidance coping had much higher suggestibility scores than those who used active coping. Howard and Hong (2002) found that emotion-focused copers scored significantly higher on suggestibility than did problem-focused copers. As the above review shows, although several studies have examined the personality correlates of false memory induced by paradigms such as the DRM and misleading questions, but few have involved false memory induced by the misinformation paradigm (see Liebman et al., 2002, for an exception). This paradigm is commonly used to elicit false memory related to eyewitness inaccuracies. It involves three standard stages: experiencing an event, receiving misinformation, and being tested for memory of the original event (Loftus, 2005). False memory is created when subjects report information from the misinformation session as part of the original event. Given that false memories induced by different paradigms are only modestly related to one another and are likely to involve different cognitive processes (Qin et al., 2008), it is important to extend the current research to include personality correlates of misinformation false memory. The current study examined the following correlates of misinformation false memory: dimensional personality factors (the Temperament and Character Inventory-Revised (TCI-R), which includes seven dimensions: harm avoidance, cooperativeness, reward dependence, self-directedness, persistence, novelty seeking, and self-transcendence), dissociation (the Dissociative Experiences Scale), depression (the Beck Depression Inventory-II), anxiety (the Beck Anxiety Inventory and the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale), and coping styles (the Brief COPE Inventory). Although previous findings were not always consistent, the preponderance of evidence led us to hypothesize that false memory from misinformation would be positively related to dissociation, depression, anxiety, and negative coping, and negatively related to active coping. In terms of dimensional personality scales, no study has used the TCI-R. Given the documented associations between dimensions of TCI-R and the NEO Personality Inventory (Cloninger, 1999), we hypothesized that false memory would be positively related to cooperativeness and reward dependence of TCI-R (based on Liebman et al.’s (2002) results on agreeableness of the NEO, which is positively related to the above two dimensions in TCI-R), and negatively to novelty seeking (based on Porter et al.’s, 2000 finding with extraversion). We did not advance specific hypotheses about other four TCI-R dimensions. 1.1.6. Interactions between personality and cognitive factors in false memory In addition to personality factors, cognitive factors also contribute to individual differences in false memory. Many studies have documented various cognitive correlates of individual differences in false memory (Gudjonsson, 2003). In our recent study (Zhu et al., submitted for publication), we found that misinformation false memory was related to a battery of cognitive abilities, including intelligence, perception, memory, and face judgment. In order to have a comprehensive understanding of individual differences in false memory, it is necessary to examine not only cognitive and personality factors separately and their additive effects, but also their interactions. Thus far, several studies have examined personality characteristics and cognitive abilities simultaneously as predictors of false memory (Liebman et al., 2002 and Salthouse and Siedlecki, 2007), but none has examined potential interactions between personality and cognitive factors. Theoretically it is possible that certain combinations of cognitive and personality factors (e.g., low cognitive abilities and high reliance on others) may exacerbate susceptibility to misinformation, whereas certain personality factors (e.g., low trust in others) may mitigate low cognitive abilities’ effects on false memory.