دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 32920
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

استفاده از زمینه های داستان برای حافظه واقعی و کاذب تعصب کودکان

عنوان انگلیسی
Using story contexts to bias children’s true and false memories
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
32920 2011 19 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Volume 108, Issue 1, January 2011, Pages 77–95

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
’ حافظه کاذب کودکان - پارادایم - نظریه فعال انجمنی - نظریه ردیابی فازی - توسعه حافظه - حافظه داستان -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Children’s false memories; DRM paradigm; Associative activation theory; Fuzzy trace theory; Memory development; Story memory
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  استفاده از زمینه های داستان برای حافظه واقعی و کاذب تعصب کودکان

چکیده انگلیسی

The effects of embedding standard Deese/Roediger–McDermott (DRM) lists into stories whose context biased interpretation either toward or away from the overall themes of the DRM lists on both true and false recognition were investigated with 7- and 11-year-olds. These biased story contexts were compared with the same children’s susceptibility to false memory illusions using the standard DRM list presentation paradigm. The results showed the usual age effects for true and false memories in the standard DRM list paradigm, where 11-year-olds exhibited higher rates of both true and false recognition compared with the 7-year-olds. Importantly, when DRM lists were embedded in stories, these age effects disappeared for true recognition. For false recognition, although developmental differences were attenuated, older children were still more susceptible to false memory illusions than younger children. These findings are discussed in terms of current theories of children’s false memories as well as the role of themes and elaboration in children’s memory development.

مقدمه انگلیسی

The Deese/Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm (Deese, 1959 and Roediger and McDermott, 1995) is perhaps the most robust procedure used to investigate the development of children’s spontaneous (as opposed to implanted or suggested) false memories (for a review, see Brainerd, Reyna, & Ceci, 2008). Here, participants are presented with a list of words (e.g., sour, candy, sugar, bitter) that are all associates of an unpresented but related concept, the critical lure (e.g., sweet). Recall and recognition tests reveal that participants frequently mistakenly identify the critical lure as having been present on the previously studied list. More important, developmentally, younger children are less susceptible than older children and adults to spontaneous false memory illusions ( Brainerd et al., 2008). There are two main explanations concerning the development of false memories in children. First, fuzzy trace theory (Brainerd et al., 2008) suggests that there are two qualitatively different memory traces that are formed in parallel: a verbatim trace that preserves surface details of studied material and a gist trace that preserves the meaning of the information that was studied. The retrieval of verbatim traces typically results in the correct recognition and recall of targets and the dismissal of semantically related critical lures. However, because these traces fade fairly rapidly, later tests of recall and recognition may rely more on gist traces that are more likely to produce meaning-related errors such as false memory illusions. Thus, according to fuzzy trace theory, a reliance on gist traces at retrieval can increase false memory rates and decrease true memory rates, whereas a reliance on verbatim traces can increase true memory rates and reduce false memory rates. More specifically, false memories can occur through one of two retrieval processes: similarity judgments between a gist trace and a possible memory candidate or phantom recollection in which strong gist traces permit memory reconstruction that includes related but unpresented items. Fuzzy trace theory predicts false recognition reversals because these two retrieval processes can give rise to both accurate and inaccurate memories. That is, under conditions of gist cuing, there should be a monotonic increase in both true and false memories, whereas under verbatim cuing, there should be a negative relationship between true and false memories. Developmentally, fuzzy trace theory contends that the DRM illusion is less robust in young children because their ability to extract and encode the global gist (theme) of a DRM list is relatively immature. As children develop, their ability to extract gist improves and, thus, they become more susceptible to these spontaneous false memory illusions (Brainerd et al., 2008). An alternative theoretical account of false memory development is the associative activation theory (Howe, 2005, Howe Wimmer, & Blease, 2009 and Howe, Wimmer, Gagnon, et al., 2009). This theory stipulates that associative activation of concepts in one’s knowledge base is responsible for false memories in children and adults. The knowledge base consists of networks of interrelated concepts whose organization changes with development and experience. There are strong links between concepts that are highly related and weak or absent links between concepts that are less related (Gallo, 2006). When a specific concept or word is encountered, its corresponding memory representation is activated, activation that can spread to surrounding related representations in the knowledge base, including theme nodes that are related to the subset of concepts being activated (see also Arndt & Reder, 2003). These theme nodes exist like other nodes in the network, and individual concepts can be linked to multiple themes (see Fig. 1). When DRM or associative lists are studied, a number of different themes are activated, and each of these themes can give rise to false recollection. Of course, lists evoking fewer themes give rise to false memories more easily than lists with more potential themes because each list member provides activation for a smaller set of potential themes (see Fig. 1 for an illustration). Recall that because these lists are associative and there can be many potential integrating themes (e.g., relations can be varied and include temporal contiguity, spatial proximity, feature overlap, shared perceptual properties, category membership, antonymy, and synonymy [see Wu & Barsalou, 2009]), lists with fewer themes stand a greater chance of activating a single theme more quickly, and this theme’s overall activation may be greater than that of lists with more themes (also see Arndt and Reder, 2003 and Reder et al., 2009). Such activation, not just backward associative strength, can increase false memory rates.