دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 34737
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

خویشتن داری غذایی، اضطراب و تقویت ارزش نسبی مواد غذایی میان وعده در زنان غیر چاق

عنوان انگلیسی
Dietary restraint, anxiety, and the relative reinforcing value of snack food in non-obese women
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
34737 2006 10 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Eating Behaviors, Volume 7, Issue 4, November 2006, Pages 323–332

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
خویشتن داری غذایی - استرس - اضطراب - تقویت ارزش غذایی - تقویت مواد غذایی - رفتار خوردن - انتخاب مواد غذایی - انتخاب رفتاری - اقتصاد رفتاری -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Dietary restraint; Stress; Anxiety; Reinforcing value of food; Food reinforcement; Eating behavior; Food choice; Behavioral choice; Behavioral economics.
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  خویشتن داری غذایی، اضطراب و تقویت ارزش نسبی مواد غذایی میان وعده در زنان غیر چاق

چکیده انگلیسی

This study tested the independent and interactive effects of anxiety and dietary restraint on the relative reinforcing value of snack food. Thirty non-obese, female university students were assigned to one of four groups based on median split scores on measures of dietary restraint and state-anxiety: low-restraint/low-anxiety (n = 7), low-restraint/high-anxiety (n = 7), high-restraint/low-anxiety (n = 9), and high-restraint/high-anxiety (n = 7). Participants were provided the choice to earn points for palatable snack foods or fruits and vegetables using a computerized concurrent schedules choice task. The behavioural cost to gain access to snack foods increased across trials, whereas the cost to gain access to fruits and vegetables was held constant across trials. The relative reinforcing value of palatable snack food in relation to fruits and vegetables was defined as the total amount of points earned for snack food. Two-way analysis of covariance, with hunger and hedonic snack food ratings as covariates, showed that dietary restraint and anxiety had a significant interactive effect on the relative reinforcing value of snack food, indicating that the effect of anxiety on snack food reinforcement is moderated by dietary restraint. Specifically, the high-anxiety/low-restraint women found snack food significantly less reinforcing than low-anxiety/low-restraint women, but no differences emerged between high- and low-anxiety women with high-restraint. Neither restraint nor anxiety had an independent effect on the relative reinforcing value of snack food. These findings indicate that anxiety may have a suppressive effect on the relative reinforcing value of snack food in low-restrained eaters, but not an enhancing effect on snack food reinforcement in high-restrained eaters. Clinical implications of these findings are discussed.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Food is very rewarding and is a primary reinforcer (Berridge, 1996 and Salamone, 1994) but by repeated association of pleasure and hedonic feelings derived from the anticipation and ingestion of palatable food, food adopts secondary reinforcing effects (Del Parigi, Chen, Salbe, Reiman, & Tataranni, 2003). There are individual differences in the relative reinforcing value of food whereby obese individuals find food more reinforcing in relation to non-food alternatives compared to non-obese persons (Saelens & Epstein, 1996) and show a stronger preference for highly palatable foods with a high fat/sugar macronutrient content (Drewnowski and Holden-Wiltse, 1992, Drewnowski et al., 1992 and Drewnowski et al., 1992). These individual differences in food reinforcement could be one of the underlying factors responsible for the sustained positive energy balance that leads to obesity (Epstein and Saelens, 2000 and Saelens and Epstein, 1996). Food deprivation is a powerful determinant of the reinforcing value of food in humans (Bulik and Brinded, 1994, Epstein et al., 2003 and Raynor and Epstein, 2003) and animals (Carr, 1996). Similarly, both acute food deprivation in the laboratory and chronic caloric restriction in the natural environment increase consummatory eating behaviour in animals (Bolles, 1965, DiBattista, 1987 and Savory, 1988) and humans (Franklin et al., 1948, Telch and Agras, 1996 and Vohs and Heatherton, 2000). One variable that has not been well studied in relation to the reinforcing value of food is stress or anxiety, despite a broad scope of research examining the effects of stress on eating behavior. Review of early research revealed that obese persons ate more under stress or when anxious than non-obese individuals, findings that were initially thought to explain the etiology of obesity (Greeno & Wing, 1994). Subsequent research, however, indicated that dietary restraint rather than obesity or weight status was more influential in stress-induced hyperphagia (Greeno & Wing, 1994). More recently, data indicate that the effects of stress/anxiety on food intake in restrained eaters may be more equivocal than previously believed (Gorman & Allison, 1995), findings that may be explained to some degree by differences in methodology, especially differences in measurement of the restraint construct (Gorman & Allison, 1995). Using the Restraint scale developed by Herman and Polivy (1980) to assess dietary restraint in adults, with few exceptions, restrained eaters ate more when anxious or feeling negative affect, while unrestrained eaters ate the same or less (Cools et al., 1992, Frost et al., 1982, Heatherton et al., 1991, Mitchell and Epstein, 1996 and Schotte et al., 1990). Using the restraint scale from the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ-R) developed by Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, and Defares (1986), however, effects of stress on eating in restrained eaters have been mixed (Oliver et al., 2000 and Wardle et al., 2000). However, when restraint was assessed using the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire – Restraint scale (TFEQ-R) in adult women, laboratory studies found that food intake was unaffected by stress/anxiety in restrainers, and either remained the same or decreased in non-restrained adult women (Haynes et al., 2003, Lowe and Maycock, 1988 and Steere and Cooper, 1993). Similarly, Weinstein, Shide, and Rolls (1997) found a significant positive relationship between change in food consumption under stress and restraint using the Restraint Scale, but the relationship did not reach statistical significance using the TFEQ-R. In trying to reconcile the inconsistent pattern of data, it has been suggested that the Restraint scale actually measures cognitive restraint in addition to dieting status and history of dieting and weight loss (Stunkard & Messick, 1985), thus represents a multidimensional measure of restraint as compared to the unidimensional construct of cognitive restraint measured by the TFEQ-R. This implies that the findings of disinhibited eating during stress may be related to other factors besides the cognitive decision to restrict eating for weight loss or weight maintenance. In fact, Stunkard and Messick (1985) point out that a person who scores high on the cognitive restraint factor (TFEQ-R) would not be overeating, unless that person also scored high on the disinhibition factor of the TFEQ. The low to moderate correlations (which range from r = .35 to .74) between the Restraint Scale and TFEQ-R provide additional support for the contention that the two measures of restraint cannot be used interchangeably and may account, at least in part, for the discrepant findings ( Gorman & Allison, 1995). Caloric restriction and restriction of certain foods are central components of dietary intervention for obesity, but food deprivation increases the relative reinforcing value of food (Carr, 1996, Epstein et al., 2003 and Raynor and Epstein, 2003) and promotes overeating (Lowe, 1993), which may make it more difficult to maintain dietary adherence required for long-term weight loss in obesity intervention. Stress, anxiety and other disinhibiting factors (i.e. alcohol, negative affect, consumption of a forbidden food) may differentially influence the rewarding value of food in restrained and non-restrained eaters. Understanding the factors and conditions that moderate the relative reinforcing value of food may yield information that improves the treatment and prevention of eating disorders and obesity given the evidence that increases in the reinforcing properties of food are associated with increases in food intake (Epstein et al., 2004). This study was designed to test the single and combined effects of anxiety and dietary restraint on the relative reinforcing value of snack food in a sample of college females. Consistent with the results from the TFEQ-R, we predicted a restraint × anxiety interaction whereby high-restrainers who felt high-anxiety would find snack food equally relatively reinforcing as high-restrainers who felt low-anxiety, but low restrainers who felt high-anxiety would find snack food less relatively reinforcing compared to low-restrainers who felt low-anxiety, indicating the effects of anxiety on food reinforcement are moderated by restraint.