دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 34782
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

خویشتن داری افسردگی عاطفه و غذایی در غذا خوردن نوجوانان پسر و دختران در غیاب گرسنگی

عنوان انگلیسی
Depressed affect and dietary restraint in adolescent boys' and girls' eating in the absence of hunger
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
34782 2015 8 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Appetite, Volume 91, 1 August 2015, Pages 343–350

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
خوردن در غیاب گرسنگی - خویشتن داری غذایی - خلق و خوی - منفی - غذا خوردن احساسی -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Eating in the absence of hunger; Dietary restraint; Mood; Negative affect; Emotional eating.
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  خویشتن داری افسردگی عاطفه و غذایی در غذا خوردن نوجوانان پسر و دختران در غیاب گرسنگی

چکیده انگلیسی

Data suggest that depressed affect and dietary restraint are related to disinhibited eating patterns in children and adults. Yet, experimental research has not determined to what extent depressed affect acutely affects eating in the absence of physiological hunger (EAH) in adolescents. In the current between-subjects experimental study, we measured EAH in 182 adolescent (13–17 y) girls (65%) and boys as ad libitum palatable snack food intake after youth ate to satiety from a buffet meal. Just prior to EAH, participants were randomly assigned to view either a sad or neutral film clip. Dietary restraint was measured with the Eating Disorder Examination. Adolescents who viewed the sad film clip reported small but significant increases in state depressed affect relative to adolescents who viewed the neutral film clip (p < .001). Yet, there was no main effect of film condition on EAH (p = .26). Instead, dietary restraint predicted greater EAH among girls, but not boys (p < .001). These findings provide evidence that adolescent girls' propensity to report restrained eating is associated with their greater disinhibited eating in the laboratory. Additional experimental research, perhaps utilizing a more potent laboratory stressor and manipulating both affective state and dietary restraint, is required to elucidate how state affect may interact with dietary restraint to influence EAH during adolescence.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) refers to consuming foods in the absence of perceived physiological hunger (Kral & Faith, 2007). Typically measured as intake of palatable snack foods after eating a meal to satiety, EAH is one behavioral manifestation of food disinhibition that may be important for obesity risk (French, Epstein, Jeffery, Blundell, & Wardle, 2012). Increased exposure to large portions of inexpensive, readily available, palatable, and energy-dense foods in contemporary Western cultures is a well-recognized contributor to positive trends in overeating behaviors that promote a positive energy balance (Hill & Peters, 1998). Indeed, overweight and obese youth (body mass index [BMI, kg/m2] ≥85th percentile for age and sex) consume more energy from palatable snack foods in the absence of hunger than youth who are not overweight (Cutting et al, 1999, Fisher, Birch, 2002, Fisher et al, 2007, Hill et al, 2008, Moens, Braet, 2007 and Shomaker et al, 2010). Despite widespread access to palatable foods, frequency of EAH varies substantially at the individual level (Kral et al, 2010 and Moens, Braet, 2007). Research is needed to understand potentially modifiable factors that explain the significant variation in EAH that persists between individuals. From a number of theoretical perspectives, depressed affect is hypothesized to play a role in disinhibited eating. Emotion regulation models posit that acute increases in negative affect trigger disinhibited eating episodes as an attempt to alleviate or escape from emotional distress (Hawkins, Clement, 1984 and Heatherton, Baumeister, 1991). Indeed, depressive symptoms are positively associated with adolescents' self-reported consumption of sugary foods and percent calories from fat (Fulkerson et al, 2004 and Kim et al, 2013), as well as the onset of disinhibited eating among girls (Skinner et al, 2012 and Stice et al, 2002). Similarly, we have found that adolescents' depressive symptoms were associated with greater observed total energy intake and greater intake of sweet snack foods at a buffet lunch meal, controlling for inter-individual variations in body composition (Mooreville et al., 2014). Data from our laboratory have also illustrated that children's and adolescents' pre-meal state depressed affect was positively correlated with energy intake at a buffet lunch meal, even after adjusting for individual differences in body composition (Ranzenhofer et al, 2013 and Vannucci et al, 2012). However, these correlational studies evaluated eating in a fasted or hungry state and the extent to which depressive symptoms and/or state depressed affect influence adolescents' eating in the absence of physiological hunger is unclear. Studies utilizing experimental methods are essential for determining to what extent state depressed mood has an acute effect on disinhibited eating. Relatively few experimental studies have evaluated the effect of negative state affect, depressive or otherwise, on EAH. Furthermore, existing data are mixed. In several studies of adults, exposure to a laboratory stressor, compared to a neutral control condition, resulted in greater EAH, particularly among overweight participants (Born et al, 2010, Lemmens et al, 2011 and Rutters et al, 2009). In contrast, among children ages 6–13 y, there was no effect of a negative or neutral mood induction on EAH in the laboratory (Hilbert, Tuschen-Caffier, & Czaja, 2010). The effect of negative affect on EAH in adolescents has not been experimentally evaluated. Adolescence is an especially salient developmental period to elucidate the potential effects of depressed affect on disinhibited eating patterns. Marked by increases in negative affect, particularly among girls (Flook, 2011 and Weinstein et al, 2007), adolescence also is an important time for increases in dietary restraint, which refers to intentions to cut back or reduce an individual's energy intake, regardless of whether a person is successful (Engelsen, 2000). According to theoretical models of restraint, efforts to limit energy intake may increase vulnerability to disinhibited overeating, including eating in the absence of hunger, when self-imposed dietary rules are violated or through heightened responsiveness to environmental food cues, regardless of whether intake is actually limited (Lowe, Levine, 2005 and Polivy, Herman, 1985). Consistent with this theoretical framework, one meta-analysis indicates that dietary restraint is a prospective risk factor for the onset of reported disinhibited eating episodes in adolescents (Stice, 2002). The relationship between dietary restraint and EAH in adolescents is not fully understood. Some laboratory eating studies suggest that the interaction of restraint and negative affect, in both state and more sustained forms, may be relevant for overeating behavior in youth. For instance, adolescents who were high in depressive symptoms and high in dietary restraint consumed the greatest energy from sweet snack foods during a laboratory buffet meal delivered after an overnight fast (Mooreville et al., 2014). Likewise, while change in total energy intake following a laboratory-induced stress (versus a neutral condition) did not differ for children (8–12 y) (Roemmich, Lambiase, Lobarinas, & Balantekin, 2011), there appears to be a significant interaction between restraint and perceived stress (Balantekin, Roemmich, 2012, Roemmich et al, 2011 and Roemmich et al, 2002). Specifically, children who were high (versus low) in both dietary restraint and perceived state stress experienced the most significant increases in total energy and snacks after a small pre-load (Balantekin, Roemmich, 2012, Roemmich et al, 2002 and Roemmich et al, 2011). Because these investigations studied youth who were fasted (Mooreville et al., 2014) or moderately hungry (Balantekin, Roemmich, 2012, Roemmich et al, 2002 and Roemmich et al, 2011), it remains unclear to what extent dietary restraint, state negative affect, and depressive symptoms are relevant to EAH in adolescents. In the current study, we sought to determine the effect of a negative mood induction on EAH in the laboratory among adolescents. We anticipated that adolescents exposed to a depressed state affect induction would consume more palatable snacks in the absence of hunger than adolescents exposed to a neutral affect induction. In addition, we aimed to evaluate the associations of depressive symptoms and dietary restraint with EAH. We expected that both factors would be positively associated with EAH, after accounting for potential demographic and anthropometric confounds such as body composition. Lastly, we evaluated the interactions among sex, depressive symptoms, dietary restraint, and state affect induction. Based upon prior research, we predicted that depressive symptoms and restraint would have a stronger impact on EAH after a depressed state affect (versus neutral) induction, and we also hypothesized that these effects might be more pronounced for girls as compared to boys (Bernier et al, 2010, de Lauzon-Guillain et al, 2009, Madowitz et al, 2014 and Nguyen-Rodriguez et al, 2009).