دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 37273
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

اثر نوزاد و سندرم مرد جوان: تأثیرات اجتماعی بر ریسک پذیری تعاونی ها در زنان و مردان

عنوان انگلیسی
The baby effect and young male syndrome: social influences on cooperative risk-taking in women and men
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
37273 2012 7 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Evolution and Human Behavior, Volume 33, Issue 5, September 2012, Pages 530–536

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
ریسک پذیری - تفاوت های جنسیتی - روانشناسی تکاملی - جفت گیری - فصلنامه علمی پژوهشی باروری-
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Risk-taking; Gender differences; Evolutionary psychology; Mating; Reproduction
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  اثر نوزاد و سندرم مرد جوان: تأثیرات اجتماعی بر ریسک پذیری تعاونی ها در زنان و مردان

چکیده انگلیسی

Parental investment theory predicts differences in risk-taking for females and males as a consequence of reproductive context, with females attempting to reduce risks in relation to their own offspring (here called the baby effect) and males taking more risks in competition with one another (young male syndrome). The experiment we report tests these predictions in a cooperative context by introducing the Social Balloon Analogue Risk Task—the Balloon Analogue Risk Task modified to include a social partner (adult male, adult female, or baby)—along with a commitment device in which participants choose among several possible social partners, with whom they will share their earnings. Results were consistent with the predictions of parental investment theory. Females did not change their levels of risk-taking when paired with adult males or females, but showed a strong reduction in risk when paired with babies. Consistent with previous research, males were strongly inclined to take more risks when paired with another male of the same age, but males showed no change in risk-taking when paired with a female of the same age or a child. The current work provides the first experimental evidence of gender differences in cooperative social risk-taking, as well as the first experimental evidence of a mediator of female risk-taking, i.e., babies.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Suppose you were out walking with a potential long-term mate, and the opportunity arose to take a bet with a street performer. Assuming you would share the winnings with your partner, do you take the bet? Now suppose, instead, that you were walking with a child when you encounter the street performer—do you take the bet? These scenarios are representative of many domains where risk-taking has social consequences. They further demonstrate that the social consequences may vary in terms of reproductive context, that is, whether the context involves a reproductive opportunity (or threat) or an instance of parental care. Though it is well accepted that, in many domains, men take more risks than women (e.g., Byrnes et al., 1999 and Weber et al., 2002), the exact reasons for this risk asymmetry are not well understood. Elegant efforts to explain these differences from an evolutionary perspective have been remarkably insightful (e.g., Baker and Maner, 2008, Baker and Maner, 2009, Daly and Wilson, 1994, Wang et al., 2009 and Wilson and Daly, 1985). Following this evolutionary approach, the current paper reports an experiment that extends these previous findings by testing two foundational predictions of parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972), in particular, that females will take fewer risks when in the presence of young children and that males will take more risks when paired with other males (even when these males are potential allies). Parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972 and Williams, 1975) attempts to explain how individuals should expend their resources to maximize their reproductive fitness. Noting that, across many species, one sex invests more (the ‘limiting sex’) and the other invests less, the theory explains that “Individuals of the sex investing less will compete among themselves to breed with members of the sex investing more, since an individual of the former can increase its reproductive success by investing successively in the offspring of several members of the limiting sex” (p. 141, Trivers, 1972). This statement lays out the two ideas central to understanding the evolution of risk-taking in humans. First, one sex (females) invests more in offspring and therefore has more to lose with the loss of any individual offspring. Second, the nonlimiting sex (males) should risk more (through competition and display) to maximize its reproductive opportunities with the limiting sex (i.e., females). Before describing our study, we discuss these evolutionary views on risk-taking individually and in more detail. 2. Parental care and female risk-avoidance The central observation on which parental investment theory is based is that the sexes invest different amounts in offspring (Trivers, 1972). For humans, females invest more in their offspring than males. This is in part due to their higher parental investment during gestation and more limited opportunity for children in the future. But it is also because the female does not share the male's paternal uncertainty or the male's opportunity costs associated with seeking additional offspring with other individuals. In the presence of offspring, we predict that this will lead females to risk less than males, who do not share the same investment. Evolutionary explanations are often based on this prediction of offspring-induced female risk-avoidance. As an example, Fessler, Pillsworth, and Flamson (2004) found that women are more sensitive to disgust and react more fearfully to situations that evoke anger in men. Providing an evolutionary explanation, Fessler et al. (2004) argued that the feeling of disgust in women signals the presence of potentially harmful influences (e.g., pathogens) that may threaten fetal development. In an experiment, they further demonstrated that women manipulated to feel disgusted expressed lower levels of risk-taking compared to women not manipulated. No such effect was found in men. Similar explanations for offspring-related risk-avoidance in females are quite common (e.g., Buss, 2004 and Fetchenhauer and Buunk, 2005). Several studies demonstrate additional indirect evidence of offspring-related risk-avoidance. Tangential evidence that females may have more reason to reduce risks associated with offspring is found in a study of parental grief following child mortality, where mothers were found to have significantly more negative feelings (e.g., despair, depression, and loss of appetite) than fathers (Schwab, 1996). This is consistent with the idea that females may perceive that they have more to lose with the loss of any individual offspring, and that postgestational parental care invested by females is thus a mechanism for reducing the risks to their offspring. More generally, Wang and colleagues (2009) have found that parents self-report lower likelihoods of engaging in risky behaviors associated with within- and between-group competition (but they do not report whether this effect was also mediated by gender). In this light, parental care can be interpreted as an offspring-related risk-avoidance policy, which—in the presence of young children—should lead to a greater reduction in risk-taking for females than males. With respect to the present experiments, it is important to note that females may also benefit from caring for offspring that are not their own. In both birds and mammals, caregivers often benefit from experience, and this may be gained through the care of young that are not genetically related (Emlen, 1984). Accordingly, many animal societies demonstrate cooperative breeding, even among nonrelated individuals. Besides experience acquired through caregiving, many of these societies show clear evidence of reciprocity (Clutton-Brock, 2002), which can further aid females in raising their own offspring. However, the benefit to males of caring for nonrelated offspring is not so apparent. Tests of the paternal resemblance hypothesis indicate that males, relative to females, are more discriminating in how they allocate parental investment—in particular, they prefer offspring that resemble themselves (Platek, Burch, Panyavin, Wasserman, & Gallup, 2002). This is consistent with the evolutionary hypothesis that males gain less from indiscriminate caregiving than females and should therefore be generally less interested in reducing risks associated with children. In sum, evolutionary theory leads to the prediction that females should be more risk-averse than males around young children—what we call the baby effect—but to our knowledge, this fundamental claim of parental investment theory has not yet been tested experimentally.