دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 39036
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

تغییرات اتونوم روزانه و واکنش پذیری عاطفی

عنوان انگلیسی
Diurnal autonomic variations and emotional reactivity
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
39036 2005 10 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Biological Psychology, Volume 69, Issue 3, July 2005, Pages 261–270

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
تنوع روزانه - فعالیت الکترودرمال - واکنش های احساسی - پاسخ رسانایی پوست - نشانگرهای جسمی
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Diurnal variability; Electrodermal activity; Emotional responses; Skin conductance responses; Somatic markers
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  تغییرات اتونوم روزانه و واکنش پذیری عاطفی

چکیده انگلیسی

Abstract Diurnal temporal variations of emotional reactivity were investigated in relationship with fluctuations of psychophysiological state, evaluated by tonic autonomic activity. Neutral and unpleasant pictures, selected to constitute seven matched sets, were evaluated by 12 participants during seven sessions in the course of daytime. Skin conductance level (SCL) was recorded at the beginning of each session, whereas skin conductance responses (SCRs), affective evaluation and emotional experience were measured during or after each session. Data show significant temporal variations for SCL, SCRs and emotional experience which follow similar patterns during the daytime. Discussion highlights the role of temporal patterns of autonomic activity as an interesting basis to better understand the emotional regulation and affective disorders.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Introduction Emotions are not a single response but rather a collection of responses including physiological activation, subjective experience and behavioral expressions. The emotional reactivity has been considered as an affective style referring to a consistent individual pattern of responses to emotional stimuli (Cacioppo et al., 1992 and Wheeler et al., 1993). However, some authors (Cacioppo and Gardner, 1999) emphasized that in similar conditions, the same individual is able to emotionally react in different ways. This intra-individual variability could proceed from both personal life events and spontaneous temporal variations of physiological systems allowing emotional manifestations. Indeed, classical data have shown that changes in autonomic activation or cognitive states preceding the onset of a stimulus, could modulate emotional responses in normal (Schachter and Singer, 1962 and Lazarus, 1985) and in brain injured subjects (Damasio, 1994). In animals, Poirel (1995) reported rhythms in both somatic and autonomic components of emotional behavior, supporting the hypothesis of two asynchronous circadian systems implicated in emotional reactivity. In addition, Poirel (1974) found out, in depressed and schizophrenic patients, similar temporal variations in emotional reactivity and vigilance, comparable to those of circadian rhythms reported some years later (Okawa et al., 1984). In spite of that, no systematic studies of the temporal organization of emotions have been performed in healthy subjects. However, several authors showed diurnal variations on tonic and phasic psychophysiological components implicated in emotional reactivity. Thus, the hypothesis of a diurnal variability of tonic emotional states is supported by an extensive body of research on mood (Thayer, 1989, Kerkhof and Van Dongen, 1996 and Owens et al., 2000). Focusing on physiological components of emotional reactivity, several authors reported spontaneous diurnal fluctuations of autonomic variables (Werntz et al., 1983 and Shannahoff-Khalsa, 1991), considered as major physiological signatures of emotional reactivity. The electrodermal activity (EDA), a largely used tonic and phasic autonomic marker of arousal dimension of emotions, presents reliable circadian periodicity throughout childhood, only few days after birth (Hellbrügge et al., 1964). Concerning the tonic EDA, usually recorded as skin conductance level (SCL), two patterns of diurnal variations have been reported in healthy adults. The former, corresponds to an increase during the morning until a peak between 1:00 p.m. and 3:00 p.m. (Venables and Christie, 1973). The latter, described by Hellbrügge et al. (1964) and more recently by Hot et al., 1999 and Hot et al., 2000, corresponds to a monotonous increase during the daytime until a peak at the evening. These data on diurnal variations of EDA raise the question about temporal relationships between the psychophysiological state sustaining the emotional activation and other indices of emotional reactivity. This view is consistent with the somatic marker hypothesis (Damasio, 1994) that emphasizes interactions between autonomic changes and emotional experience. In this frame, mood disorders have been considered as the consequence of temporal dysregulations of physiological systems allowing the tonic expression of emotions (Wirz-Justice and Van den Hoofdakker, 1999). Then, the research of temporal patterning between components of emotional reactivity in healthy subjects could have a particular relevance in the comprehension of affective disorders. The aim of this study was to investigate diurnal organization of emotional reactivity components, and to assess its relationships with diurnal variations of tonic psychophysiological state. To this end, electrodermal activity, affective evaluation and emotional experience were recorded before or during the presentation of emotional pictures through several sessions in the course of daytime.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

Results The pre-stimulus SCL showed significant diurnal variations (F(6,66) = 3.40, p < 0.05) stemmed from a linear trend (F(1,6) = 5.51, p < 0.05). Such result indicates that SCL increased during the day ( Fig. 2A). The observed SCL peak at 3:30 p.m. did not differ significantly from the other daytime values in the post hoc comparisons. Diurnal variations of mean skin conductance level (SCL, panel (A)), mean ... Fig. 2. Diurnal variations of mean skin conductance level (SCL, panel (A)), mean amplitude of skin conductance responses (SCRs) in unpleasant and neutral conditions (B) and mean values of emotional experience obtained by the z-transformation of individual data (C). Error bars represent the standard error to the mean. Figure options A significant pictures × time of day effect is observed (F(6,66) = 2.67, p < 0.05) for SCRs ( Fig. 2B). Contrasts analysis indicated that SCRs to unpleasant stimulus presented a significant linear trend (F(1,11) = 5.52, p < 0.05) but not quadratic trend (F(1,11) = 1.54, ns), whereas SCRs to neutral ones present a significant quadratic trend (F(1,11) = 4.65, p < 0.05) but no linear one (F(1,11) = 0.36, ns). Unpleasant pictures were judged significantly different from neutral ones for both valence (SAM mean ratings and S.E.M. for neutral and unpleasant pictures are respectively 5.4 (0.44) and 2.6 (0.56); F(1,11) = 324.92, p < 0.001) and arousal (neutral: 3.9 (0.38); unpleasant: 6.9 (0.25); F(1,11) = 65.82, p < 0.001). Temporal factor was tested for valence and arousal dimensions of neutral and unpleasant pictures: for the whole set of data, the null hypothesis can not be rejected (valence: F(6,66) = 1.11, ns; arousal: F(6,66) = 0.50, ns), i.e., SAM responses seemed to be stable across daytime. Concerning the self-reported emotional impact of sessions, participants have not used in the same way the visual analogue scales. First, most of subjects have only reported their emotional experience on some preferential scales during all the experiment. Second, when a particular scale was considered, some participants indicated feelings values throughout the whole line whereas others only used a small portion of the same line. Then, we used a global measure of emotional experience obtained by the z-transformation of individual data and averaging values of all the scales. As showed in Fig. 2C, a daytime effect (F(6,66) = 2.25, p < 0.05) stemmed from a quadratic trend (F(1,11) = 5.40, p < 0.05) with a peak around 3:30 p.m. In order to examine temporal relationships between indicators of emotional reactivity and the tonic psychophysiological state, an additional analysis was performed on measurements which presented diurnal variations. We found that variations in SCL were significantly positively correlated with SCRs to unpleasant and neutral pictures (respectively r = 0.81, p < 0.05; r = 0.83, p < 0.05). This temporal pattern was confirmed by individual temporal correlations (9 participants for unpleasant pictures and seven participants for neutral ones, see Table 1). Moreover, emotional experience fluctuations were positively correlated with SCRs to unpleasant pictures (r = 0.88, p < 0.05; eight subjects) and in trend with SCL (r = 0.72, p < 0.10; seven subjects). Table 1. Individual temporal correlations between SCL or SCRs elicited either by unpleasant (Un SCRs) or neutral (Nt SCRs) pictures and emotional experience scores (EE) *p < 0.10 and **p < 0.05