دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 32584
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

کمال گرایی و افسردگی عاطفی: جوانب مثبت و منفی بودن کمال گرا

عنوان انگلیسی
Perfectionism and depressive affect: the pros and cons of being a perfectionist1
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
32584 1999 14 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Personality and Individual Differences, Volume 26, Issue 3, 1 March 1999, Pages 549–562

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
کمال گرایی - افسردگی عاطفی - حوادث استرس زا در زندگی - مدیر -
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
perfectionism; depressive affect; stressful life events; moderator
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  کمال گرایی و افسردگی عاطفی: جوانب مثبت و منفی بودن کمال گرا

چکیده انگلیسی

Recent research indicates that perfectionism may be best viewed as a number of distinct dimensions which are differentially related to depressive affect. The first hypothesis was that an increasing tendency for individuals to procrastinate because they fear making a mistake (i.e. passive perfectionism) would be related to higher levels of depressive affect, while an increasing tendency for individuals to strive for achievement (i.e. active perfectionism) would be unrelated to higher levels of depressive affect. The second hypothesis was that passive perfectionism, but not active perfectionism, would moderate the impact of stressful life events upon depressive affect. Both hypotheses were supported. Moreover, higher levels of organisational perfectionism may actually reduce the levels of depressive affect because higher levels of organisational perfectionism were found to correlate with lower levels of depressive affect. Overall, being a perfectionist may be beneficial as well as detrimental when considering the relationship between perfectionism and depressive affect.

مقدمه انگلیسی

Depressive disorders have been viewed as the “common cold” of psychopathology (Rosenhan and Seligman, 1995). Moreover, there is additional evidence that the severity and frequency of depressive symptoms in Western countries is increasing because there has been a tenfold increase in the rates of depression diagnosed in the United States over the last 2–3 generations (Robins et al., 1984). Understandably, a major focus of research in recent years has been to specify the range of personality factors that may predispose individuals to experience high levels of depressive affect (e.g. Beck, 1967; Peterson and Seligman, 1984; Abramson et al., 1989). Perfectionism is one aspect of personality which has been viewed as a factor involved in the aetiology of depressive symptoms and a growing body of evidence supports the proposal that individuals who are highly perfectionistic also tend to display higher levels of depressive affect (see reviews by Blatt, 1995; Flett et al., 1995). Flett et al. (1995)have noted, however, that research designed to investigate the relationship between perfectionism and depressive affect has been limited by the assumption that perfectionism is a unidimensional rather than a multidimensional construct. Frost et al. (1990)addressed the need for a multidimensional measure of perfectionism by developing the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS). The MPS provides a global measure of perfectionism as well as ratings of perfectionism along the six dimensions of concern over mistakes (CM), personal standards (PS), parental expectations (PE), parental criticism (PC), doubts about actions (D) and organisation (O). A particular benefit of the MPS is an opportunity to investigate the possibility of differential relationships between perfectionism and depressive affect (see Flett et al., 1995; Frost et al., 1990). Given the assumption that depressive affect and suicidal preoccupation are closely related, Adkins and Parker (1996)provided evidence to indicate that differential relationships could emerge between perfectionism and depressive affect when a multidimensional measure of perfectionism was used. Adkins and Parker (1996)found, for instance, that the CM (concern over mistakes) and D (doubt about actions) subscales but not the PS (personal standards), PE (parental expectations), PC (parental criticism) or O (organisation) subscales of the MPS were related to suicidal preoccupation in a canonical correlation analysis. Adkins and Parker (1996)examined the items in the MPS and formulated a post-hoc explanation for the differential relationships between perfectionism and suicidal preoccupation. The CM (concern over mistakes) and D (doubt about actions) subscales were associated with suicidal preoccupation because the two subscales measured “passive perfectionism”, while the PS (personal standards), PE (parental expectations), PC (parental criticism) and O (organisation) subscales were not associated with suicidal preoccupation because they measured “active perfectionism”. Adkins and Parker (1996), p. 539) proposed that high levels of passive perfectionism denoted “individuals who are inordinately afraid of making mistakes, who frequently second-guess their own decisions, who procrastinate, and for whom perfectionism creates impediments to action”. In contrast, Adkins and Parker (1996), p. 539) proposed that high levels of active perfectionism indicated individuals “who appear not to be predisposed to suicidal preoccupation, are those for whom perfectionistic strivings motivate rather than paralyze; for whom perfectionism spurs rather than inhibits achievement”. The relationship between passive perfectionism and depressive affect as outlined by Adkins and Parker (1996)can also be understood within the framework provided by current theories of depression such as hopelessness theory (Abramson et al., 1989) and Beck's theory of depression (Beck, 1967). Both theories of depression speculate that hopelessness about controlling desired or aversive outcomes is a central factor in the aetiology of depressive affect. An examination of the two theories of depression and the speculations by Adkins and Parker (1996)reveals that there is considerable overlap between the concept of hopelessness and the concept of passive perfectionism. The overlap occurs because both the concept of hopelessness and the concept of passive perfectionism place a strong emphasis on beliefs about the inability to stop aversive events. In the case of passive perfectionism, for instance, aversive events refers to fears that making a mistake is unavoidable. Accordingly, the speculation that passive perfectionism and depressive affect are related concords with current theories of depression because passive perfectionism may represent a form of hopelessness which is particularly prevalent in some individuals who have a tendency towards perfectionism. The speculations by Adkins and Parker (1996)and the current speculations about the role of hopelessness provides an opportunity to develop a greater theoretical understanding of the differential relationships between perfectionism and depressive affect. Nevertheless, the current formulation does assume that the differential relationships reported by Adkins and Parker (1996)between perfectionism and suicidal preoccupation also applies to the differential relationships between perfectionism and depressive affect. Before examining additional aspects of the current formulation concerning the differential relationships between perfectionism and depressive affect, the findings reported by Adkins and Parker (1996)using an outcome measure of suicidal preoccupation need to be reproduced using an outcome measure of depressive affect. The first hypothesis in the present study, therefore, was to test the prediction that the differential relationships between perfectionism and suicidal preoccupation would be maintained when depressive affect was used as the outcome measure instead of suicidal preoccupation. The speculations by Adkins and Parker (1996)also provide an opportunity to examine an assumption prevalent in the research literature that perfectionists are vulnerable or predisposed to experience depressive disorders during periods of high stress (e.g. Burns, 1980; Pacht, 1984; Blatt, 1995). Although a considerable amount of research has examined the ability of perfectionism to predict or correlate with depressive affect, few studies have tested the assumption that perfectionism represents a vulnerability factor which increases the chances that an individual may experience high levels of depressive affect when exposed to high levels of threatening life events (Flett et al., 1995). Two recent studies by Hewitt and Flett (1993)and Hewitt et al. (1996)have supported the speculation that perfectionists are vulnerable to experience depressive disorders during periods of high stress. However, the studies by Hewitt and colleagues did not address the speculations by Adkins and Parker (1996)because the measures of perfectionism could not evaluate active and passive perfectionism. The second focus of the present study, therefore, was to address the need for research that examines the vulnerability of perfectionistic individuals to experience depressive disorders during periods of high stress by examining some of the speculations concerning passive and active perfectionism outlined by Adkins and Parker (1996). Adkins and Parker (1996)examined the role of perfectionism as a vulnerability factor in the development of depressive symptoms with the prediction that passive perfectionism, but not active perfectionism, moderates the impact of stressful life events upon depressive affect. Although Adkins and Parker (1996)did not provide a detailed account of the psychological processes on which the moderation hypothesis was based, the definitions of passive and active perfectionism provides the foundations for a plausible scenario. In particular, passive perfectionism would moderate the impact of stressful life events upon depressive affect because individuals who are higher on passive perfectionism would be increasingly fearful of making a mistake and more prone to higher levels of depressive affect as the levels of stressful life events increased. On the other hand, active perfectionism would not moderate the impact of stressful life events upon depressive affect because active perfectionism would evaluate the drive for achievement that is independent of any fears about making a mistake. Overall, the second hypothesis in the present study was that passive perfectionism, but not active perfectionism, moderates the impact of stressful life events upon depressive affect.