دانلود مقاله ISI انگلیسی شماره 34654
ترجمه فارسی عنوان مقاله

استدلال هدف به عنوان یک شکل کلی از فراشناخت در BICA

عنوان انگلیسی
Goal reasoning as a general form of metacognition in BICA
کد مقاله سال انتشار تعداد صفحات مقاله انگلیسی
34654 2014 18 صفحه PDF
منبع

Publisher : Elsevier - Science Direct (الزویر - ساینس دایرکت)

Journal : Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, Volume 9, July 2014, Pages 105–122

ترجمه کلمات کلیدی
معماری شناختی - استدلال هدف - استقلال - عوامل هوشمند
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی
Cognitive architectures; Goal reasoning; Autonomy; Intelligent agents
پیش نمایش مقاله
پیش نمایش مقاله  استدلال هدف به عنوان یک شکل کلی از فراشناخت در BICA

چکیده انگلیسی

A key capability that distinguishes humans from intelligent agents is the ability to generate and select new goals in an unexpected situation, while re-prioritizing existing goals. This level of cognitive autonomy becomes practically vital for actors working as team members with humans or in unpredictable environments. The work is focused on a new perspective on this problem based on biologically inspired cognitive architectures (BICA). Aspects of goal reasoning and goal management capabilities that are currently available in selected BICA are reviewed. A generalizing model of goal reasoning as a form of metacognition is defined, that integrates multiple mechanisms. On top of the traditional cognitive cycle, the model includes a goal-reasoning cycle triggered by notable phenomena. This goal reasoning cycle integrates mechanisms like agitation of desires by drives and their instantiation in goals, or metacognitive reasoning about goals. The model is illustrated by consideration of specific scenarios at micro- and macro-cognitive scales to evaluate the use and potential capabilities of this goal reasoning model for BICA. New capabilities that will be enabled based on this approach are expected to increase the human compatibility and cognitive autonomy of future intelligent agents. The general goal reasoning capability is a key component included in the BICA Challenge and is required for integration of intelligent agents into human teams.

مقدمه انگلیسی

To integrate into human teams as teammates, future intelligent agents will need certain general capabilities that will make them human-compatible, and therefore believable and acceptable to humans as reliable partners (Samsonovich, 2012). For example, these artificial actors1 will need to create and manage their goals, model and reason about their own and others’ beliefs, reason about human emotions, and acquire knowledge about new tasks and environments. This work focuses on goal reasoning, while keeping in mind that all aforementioned components of higher human-level intelligence are mutually dependent and need to be integrated in their study. Like most modern paradigms in artificial intelligence (AI), goal reasoning does not refer to something radically new in terms of theoretical approaches and methodology, but rather offers an original perspective on familiar approaches. However, this perspective challenges traditional ways of thinking about AI at their foundation. In a traditional understanding, AI tools are developed to aid humans in solving given tasks, specifically, tasks that require human-level intelligence and rationality (Russell & Norvig, 2003). In goal reasoning, this tacit basic assumption is turned upside down. When a goal-reasoning actor is given a certain goal, it may start investigating questions like “Why should I pursue this goal?” or “Should I pursue another goal instead?” As a result, this actor may abandon the given goal, formulate a different task for itself, and start performing it. This paradigm may seem strange, useless, and potentially dangerous. At first, it is not obvious that this goal-reasoning actor can assist the user. But perhaps a bigger concern is that uncontrolled autonomous goal reasoning could be harmful. In any case, the task of designing a useful goal-reasoning actor does not seem well-formulated or well-understood, because its definition requires metrics for success. On the one hand, it is difficult to define general metrics for goal reasoning, because it is difficult to construct a fully fleshed-out mapping of actor’s states to goals that would guarantee correct behavior. It might be impossible to provide a complete mapping of this sort for a class of real-world situations. On the other hand, if a precise mapping were given, then there would be no goal-reasoning task to solve. And without having precise and objective metrics, it seems impossible to study the problem scientifically. Yet humans frequently encounter and successfully respond to most practically meaningful and vital goal-reasoning challenges encountered in real life without a priori associated goals or rigorously defined metrics for success. The spectrum of such real-life situations can be illustrated with examples given below. (1) Imagine that you are entering a grocery store with the goal to buy some food. As you are opening the door (suppose it is not automatic), you see a person approaching from the other side who is carrying groceries in both hands. Will you hold the door for her – or continue immediately toward your goal? Although typical goal-changing behaviors like the one normally expected in this situation could be learned through education (in humans) or pre-programmed (in robots), it would be useful to have an artificial actor that can discover and solve such goal-reasoning situations “on the fly”. (2) Imagine that you are a student solving exam problems in a classroom. Suddenly an earthquake starts, and an expensive computer starts falling from the table. What will you do: try to save the computer, run for safety, or continue solving problems? In many cases like this example, the rational analysis and evaluation of the choice of behavior comes afterwards, yet humans can decide what to do immediately, in most cases correctly, and on their own, being driven by common sense. (3) An important aspect of goal reasoning is metacognition that operates on the goal states represented in the actor’s mind. This process may not be triggered by a particular exogenous event. For example, imagine that you try to navigate in the forest in heavy fog, and have no compass. You set the goal to move north, yet keep unexpectedly returning to previously visited landmarks. As a result, you re-evaluate your ability to navigate in the fog and change your current goal accordingly. (4) In many cases goal reasoning involves learning: not only learning of what goal should be pursued in a given state, but also learning in a broader sense. Imagine that you enter an unknown videogame environment, where you need to discover objects, rules of the game, possible actions and their consequences, and rewards through trial and error, before you can learn a system of values in this environment and dependencies among possible states and various conditions, together resulting in a hierarchy of goals, from immediate and concrete targets to long-term and more abstract goals. An artificial actor in a similar situation may need to infer and learn values and goals, moving step-by-step through many levels. To constructively address these and related general questions, the rest of the paper is organized as follows. Section ‘Background on goal reasoning and cognitive architectures’ provides relevant background knowledge on cognitive architectures and on goal reasoning, ending with a brief summary of specific goal-reasoning approaches and capabilities implemented in cognitive architecture. Section ‘Integrated model of goal reasoning in BICA’ attempts to construct a generic, integrated and unifying framework for design and implementation of goal reasoning capabilities in BICA. The general framework is further tested using example scenarios. Finally, Section ‘Comparative analysis of goal reasoning capabilities offered by selected cognitive architectures’ performs comparative analysis of goal reasoning capabilities available in several selected popular cognitive architectures, and summarizes results in a table. Future promising directions of research in this area and connections to other components of the BICA Challenge are discussed in the last section.

نتیجه گیری انگلیسی

There are at least three big paradigms in computational research that in effect all pursue one and the same grand challenge, yet from different perspectives and with seemingly different objectives. One of them is AI (McCarthy, Minsky, Rochester, & Shannon, 1955/2000) aiming at developing tools and machines that can solve human-level intelligent tasks. Another is cognitive modeling (where cognitive architectures belong) attempting to understand how the mind works. The third one is computational and cognitive neuroscience trying to understand how the brain works. The point of the approach known as biologically inspired cognitive architectures (BICA) is in providing an alternative view of each, while at the same time integrating them all. Therefore, typically BICA are not couched in a mathematical logic formalism, are not intended as biologically realistic models, and in many cases do not aim at precise reproduction of the limitations of human cognition. In the author’s view, these are strengths and not weaknesses of the approach based on BICA. The idea of goal reasoning is not new for any of the three fields unified in BICA: for AI, cognitive or neurosciences (regarding the latter see, e.g., Mars, Sallet, Rushworth, & Yeung, 2011). However, the paradigm of goal reasoning has the potential to become a part of the new dominant approach in the field of BICA research. Examples of dominance of selected scientific paradigms are continuous through the history of computational sciences, and include neural networks, genetic algorithms, Bayesian reasoning, and many more. Goal reasoning has a chance to become one of them, and fits particularly well with the general framework of BICA, as shown in this work. In particular, the present work contributes a high-level integrated model of goal reasoning that is suitable for BICA implementations and is based on a metacognitive loop. Several examples presented here illustrated that goal reasoning may not involve metacognition, at least explicitly. It turns out, however, that all such examples have limitations as methods of goal reasoning. At the same, the general metacognitive framework for goal reasoning formulated here in principle could be useful virtually in any circumstances when goal reasoning would make sense. This is illustrated with selected examples. In conclusion, the present work addressed various aspects of goal reasoning and goal management capabilities that are currently available in selected BICA and in other computational models. A general model of goal reasoning was formulated based on the notion of an optimal working narrative. On top of the traditional cognitive cycle, the model includes a goal-reasoning loop, that was further instantiated in two versions: one, involving agitation of desires by drives and their subsequent instantiation in goals, potentially resulting in generation of new goals, and another, involving detection of notable phenomena and metacognitive reasoning about them involving deliberation and simulation of expectations, also leading to a goal formulation. It was argued that the two mechanisms can be easily integrated using their alignment based on the common general template. The usability and potential value of the introduced general goal reasoning model (Section ‘A formalization of goal reasoning’) for BICA still need to be addressed by future empirical studies. For now, preliminary indications can be drawn from the presented tests of the model by analysis of example scenarios. They indicate that the model has a potential value for computational implementations. It is also sufficiently general and robust to be useful in a broad spectrum of applications. In addition, the model is inspired by psychological and neurophysiological facts. Therefore, new capabilities that will be enabled based on this approach can be expected to increase the human compatibility, not only cognitive autonomy of future intelligent actors and robots, thereby advancing us toward a solution of the BICA Challenge.